Join over 600,000 people saving time

Summarize, analyze and organize your research

Summarize anything

Understand complex research

Organize your knowledge

research paper flash card

AI powered tools built specifically for academic papers

From undergrad to postgrad and beyond.

Researchers

“It would normally take me 15mins – 1 hour to skim read the article but with Scholarcy I can do that in 5 minutes.”

research paper flash card

Omar Ng , Masters student ‍ @omarng

‍ It’s time to revolutionize your research workflow

So, you have texts coming at you from every angle and need to articulate your understanding of them tomorrow? We’ve been there…

research paper flash card

Summarize any paper, article or textbook.

You can summarize videos too! Scholarcy converts long complex texts into interactive summary flashcards, which highlight key information.

research paper flash card

We’re compatible!

Import any file, from anywhere. Whether you're browsing articles online, have a chapter downloaded, or a folder of PDFs and Word docs.

Enhance your summaries

Change the summary to match your reading style with our Enhance feature. Choose from a single sentence to a researcher level overview.

Jump to key findings with Spotlight

We’ll take you straight to the important points, key concepts, and contributions. 

Critically evaluate complex texts more easily

Smart highlighting and analyzing features guide you to important sections of text and help you interpret them.

research paper flash card

Find order from chaos

Our Flashcards provide a structured, consistent format to read and explore any text from, whether you’re reading just one article or 20.

Highlight, annotate and take notes

Never lose another flash of inspiration. Add notes while you read and pick up right where you left off.

Explore new concepts and terms as you go

We’ll point you to further reading and show you how the article compares to earlier work.

Keep track of your knowledge

Never lose another text. Scholarcy is the perfect tool for saving, organising and getting a quick refresher of your reading.

Save summaries and never lose another paper

Generate and save flashcards to your library even while browsing and reading on the go.

Keep track of important details

Store all of your references, figures and tables and easily find them again.

Refresh your memory

Quickly remind yourself of the key facts and findings before a lecture or meeting with your supervisor.

Synthesize your insights. Export to other apps.

Export your flashcards to a range of file formats that are compatible with lots of research and productivity apps.

research paper flash card

Export summaries to a range of formats

Learn more about your texts and how they compare, or connect by exporting to Excel, PKMS and more.

Import directly from Zotero

Convert your Zotero library into Scholarcy Flashcards for more efficient article screening.

Generate bibliographies in a click

Export your flashcards to your favourite citation manager or generate a one-click, fully formatted bibliography in Word.

Apply what you’ve learned. Write that magnum opus 🤌

Transform all that knowledge you’ve built up into a perfectly articulated argument.

Introduction

research paper flash card

Try Scholarcy today

research paper flash card

A new flashcard experience is coming

Article Summarizer

Drop files here, or

Click to import

This study has demonstrated that higher relative humidity and wind speed, and lower atmospheric pressure, were associated with increased pain severity in people with long-term pain conditions

Explore this study

Change view

There were 9695 hazard periods included in the analysis for the final 2658 participants, matched to 81,727 control periods in 6431 participant-months

Time spent outside did not have a significant interaction with relative humidity or wind speed , nor did it lead to significant associations for temperature when conducting analyses stratified by time spent outside (Supplementary Table 3 )

This study has demonstrated that higher relative humidity and wind speed , and lower atmospheric pressure, were associated with increased pain severity in people with long-term pain conditions

The ‘worst’ combination of weather variables would increase the odds of a pain event by just over 20% compared to an average day

We showed that Cloudy participants were largely representative of a population reporting chronic-pain symptoms,[13] proportionally fewer participants at both extremes of age were recruited

The odds of a pain event was 12% higher per one standard deviation increase in relative humidity (9 percentage points) ( OR 1.119 (1.084–1.154), compared to 4% lower for pressure ( OR 0.958 (0.930–0.989) and 4% higher for wind speed ( OR 1.041 (1.010–1.073) (11 mbar and 2 m s−1, respectively)

The analysis has demonstrated significant relationships between relative humidity , pressure, wind speed and pain, with correlations remaining even when accounting for mood and physical activity

This study used a smartphone app to collect data from 2658 patients with chronic pain over 15 months, finding significant relationships between pain and relative humidity, pressure, and wind speed, with correlations remaining even when accounting for mood and physical activity.

Key findings

The study found significant relationships between pain and relative humidity , pressure, and wind speed , with correlations remaining even when accounting for mood and physical activity . Relative humidity had the strongest association with pain, and temperature the least.

The study found significant relationships between relative humidity , pressure, wind speed , and pain, with correlations remaining even when accounting for mood and physical activity .

The objective of the study was to examine the relationship between local weather and daily pain in people living with long-term pain conditions using a smartphone app.

The study used a smartphone app to collect daily data from participants over 15 months, including pain symptoms, mood, physical activity , and weather data from nearby weather stations.

The study used a case-crossover design, where participants served as their own control, eliminating confounding by time-invariant factors. Weather data were obtained by linking hourly smartphone GPS data to the nearest of 154 possible United Kingdom Met Office weather stations.

The study uses a case-crossover design and statistical analysis, including the calculation of odds ratios using the following equation: n Odds Ratio = exp(βT(temperature - μT) + βRH( relative humidity - μRH) + βwsp( wind speed - μwsp) + βP(pressure - μp)).

The study found significant relationships between pain and relative humidity , pressure, and wind speed , with correlations remaining even when accounting for mood and physical activity . The odds of a pain event were higher with an increase in relative humidity and wind speed , and lower with an increase in atmospheric pressure.

The study found that the odds ratio for a pain event increased with relative humidity , pressure, and wind speed . The results remained significant even when accounting for mood and physical activity .

Conclusions

The study demonstrated that higher relative humidity and wind speed , and lower atmospheric pressure, were associated with increased pain severity in people with long-term pain conditions. The effect of weather on pain was not fully explained by its day-to-day effect on mood or physical activity .

The study concludes that there is a significant relationship between weather and pain, and that understanding this relationship is important for patients with chronic pain .

Front matter

How the weather affects the pain of citizen scientists using a smartphone app William G . Dixon 1,2,3*, Anna L . Beukenhorst 1, Belay B . Yimer[1], Louise Cook[1], Antonio Gasparrini 4,5, Tal El-Hay 6, Bruce Hellman 7, Ben James[7], Ana M. Vicedo-Cabrera[4], Malcolm Maclure[8], Ricardo Silva 9,10, John Ainsworth 2, Huai Leng Pisaniello1,[11], Thomas House 12,13, Mark Lunt 1, Carolyn Gamble3,[14,15], Caroline Sanders[14,15], David M. Schultz 16, Jamie C. Sergeant 1,3,17,18 and John McBeth1,[3,18]

Patients with chronic pain commonly believe their pain is related to the weather. Scientific evidence to support their beliefs is inconclusive, in part due to difficulties in getting a large dataset of patients frequently recording their pain symptoms during a variety of weather conditions. Smartphones allow the opportunity to collect data to overcome these difficulties. Our study Cloudy with a Chance of Pain analysed daily data from 2658 patients collected over a 15-month period . The analysis demonstrated significant yet modest relationships between pain and relative humidity, pressure and wind speed, with correlations remaining even when accounting for mood and physical activity . This research highlights how citizen-science experiments can collect large datasets on real-world populations to address long-standing health questions. These results will act as a starting point for a future system for patients to better manage their health through pain forecasts .

INTRODUCTION

Weather has been thought to affect symptoms in patients with chronic disease since the time of Hippocrates over 2000 years ago.[1] Around three-quarters of people living with arthritis believe their pain is affected by the weather .[2,3] Many report their pain is made worse by the cold, rain , and low atmospheric pressure. Others report that their pain is made worse by warmth and high humidity. Despite much research examining the existence and nature of the weather–pain relationship,[4] there remains no scientific consensus. Studies have failed to reach consensus in part due to their small sample sizes or short durations (commonly fewer than 100 participants or one month or less); by considering a limited range of weather conditions; and heterogeneity in study design (e.g. the populations studied, methods for assessing pain, assumptions to determine the weather exposure, and statistical analysis techniques).[5–11] Resolving this question requires collection of high-quality symptom and weather data on large numbers of individuals. Such data also need to include other factors potentially linked to daily pain variation and weather , such as mood and amount of physical activity . Collecting this kind of multi-faceted data in large populations over long periods of time, however, has been difficult.

The increasing uptake of smartphones offers new and significant opportunities for health research.[12] Smartphones allow the integration of data collection into daily life using applications (apps). Furthermore, embedded technologies within the smartphones, such as the Global Positioning System ( GPS ), can be used to link the data collection to specific locations. We created Cloudy with a Chance of Pain,[13,14] a national United Kingdom smartphone study, to collect a large dataset to examine the relationship between local weather and daily pain in people living with longterm pain conditions.

The study app was downloaded by 13,207 users over the 12month recruitment period (Figs 1 and 2a) with recruitment from all 124 UK postcode areas. A total of 10,584 participants had complete baseline information and at least one pain entry, with 6850 (65%) participants remaining in the study beyond their first week and 4692 (44%) beyond their first month (Fig. 2b) . Further description of engagement clusters is provided in Supplementary Table 2 and Supplementary Figs 1–3. A total of 2658 participants had at least one hazard period matched to a control period in the same month (Fig. 3) and were included in the final analysis. There were 9695 hazard periods included in the analysis for the final 2658 participants, matched to 81,727 control periods in 6431 participant-months. A total of 1235 participants contributed one month, and the remaining 1423 participants contributed 2–15 months.

The final cohort was active for a median of 165 days (interquartile range, IQR 84–245) with symptoms submitted on an average of 73% of all days. Cohort members were

How sƟff did you feel on waking No sƟffness Very severely this morning?

predominantly female (83%), had a mean age of 51 years (standard deviation 12.6), and had a range of different pain conditions, predominantly arthritis (Supplementary Table 1 ). The median number of weather stations associated with each participant during the course of their active data-collection period was 9 (IQR 4–14) with a maximum of 82 stations, indicating how mobile participants were during the course of the study and the importance of accounting for the weather at different locations over the course of the study. As an illustration of the structure of the data, the proportion of participants reporting a pain event was plotted as a heat map per calendar day for the study period (Fig. 4), aligned with the average United Kingdom weather data for the same time period. On any given day during the study, about 1–6% of participants had a pain event. At the start of the study, most participants believed in an association between weather and their pain (median score 8 out of 10, IQR 6–9). The demographics, health conditions and baseline beliefs of the 2658 participants included in the analysis were representative of the 10,584 participants who downloaded the app and provided baseline information (Supplementary Table 2 ).

The multivariable case-crossover analysis including the four state weather variables demonstrated that an increase in relative humidity was associated with a higher odds of a pain event with an OR of 1.139 (95% confidence interval 1.099–1.181) per 10 percentage point increase, as was an increase in wind speed with an OR of 1.02 (1.005–1.035) per 1 m s−1 increase ( Table 1 ). The odds of a pain event was lower with an increase in atmospheric pressure with an OR of 0.962 (0.937–0.987) per 10-mbar increase. Temperature did not have a significant association with pain ( OR 0.996 (0.985–1.007) per 1 °C increase). The odds of a pain event was 12% higher per one standard deviation increase in relative humidity (9 percentage points) (OR 1.119 (1.084–1.154), compared to 4% lower for pressure (OR 0.958 (0.930–0.989) and 4% higher for wind speed (OR 1.041 (1.010–1.073) (11 mbar and 2 m s−1, respectively) . Of the four weather variables, relative humidity had the strongest association with pain , and temperature the least, evidenced by the estimated relative importance of the variables and their standardized odds ratios ( Table 1 , Supplementary Table 4 ). Similar effect sizes were seen when each variable was examined in univariable analyses . Precipitation was not associated with an increased odds of a pain event ( OR 0.996 (0.989–1.003) per 1 mm daily rainfall amount) (Supplementary Table 5 ). Exploratory analyses considered time spent outside by including an interaction term with temperature, relative humidity , and wind speed . Time spent outside did not have a significant interaction with relative humidity or wind speed , nor did it lead to significant associations for temperature when conducting analyses stratified by time spent outside (Supplementary Table 3 ). It thus was not included in the final model.

The model was then expanded to include mood and physical activity on the day of interest, included as binary variables ( Table 1 ), resulting in a modest reduction in the point estimates for all weather variables. Mood was strongly and independently associated with pain events ( OR 4.083 (3.824–4.360) for low mood versus good mood), whereas there was no significant association with physical activity ( OR 0.939 (0.881–1.002) for high versus low activity).

This multivariable regression model output represents the effect of one weather variable while all else remains constant. In reality, a single weather variable rarely changes in isolation while others remain unchanged . To illustrate the composite effect of the weather variables on the odds of reporting pain, an OR was calculated for each day using the coefficients of our multivariable model and daily UK mean weather values. Figure 5 demonstrates there is significant variability in the odds of a pain event for any given value of each weather variable. For example, at a temperature of 8 °C, the odds of a pain event varied from around npj Digital Medicine (2019) 105

0.8–1.2, depending on the other state variables in the weather that day.

Other factors such as day of the week (Supplementary Table 6 ), lagged weather values (Supplementary Table 7 ) and changes in weather variables from the previous day were tested. Mondays, Thursdays, and Saturdays (ORs 1.14, 1.14, and 1.29, respectively) had higher odds of pain compared to Sundays, but adjusting for the day of the week did not alter the effect of the four main weather variables . Except for relative humidity (1-day lag and 2day lag), no significant associations were observed between lagged weather variables and pain events . Including change in weather from yesterday showed a minor effect of changing relative humidity ( OR 1.005 (1.001–1.009) per 10 percentage point increase), whereas the effects of today’s relative humidity and pressure remained unchanged (Supplementary Table 8 ). Stratification by disease led to a loss of statistical power and largely inconclusive results, although relative humidity appeared to have a stronger association with pain in patients with osteoarthritis (Supplementary Table 9 , Supplementary Fig. 4). Stratification by the number of pain sites also showed no meaningful difference (Supplementary Table 10 ). After stratifying by participants’ prior beliefs about their weather–pain relationship, relative humidity remained associated with pain in all participants although the association with pressure was only seen in those with a strong prior belief (Supplementary Table 11 ).

This study has demonstrated that higher relative humidity and wind speed , and lower atmospheric pressure, were associated with increased pain severity in people with long-term pain conditions . The most significant contribution was from relative humidity . The effect of weather on pain was not fully explained by its day-to-day effect on mood or physical activity . The overall effect sizes, while statistically significant, were modest. For example, the ‘worst’ combination of weather variables would increase the odds of a pain event by just over 20% compared to an average day . Nonetheless, such an increased risk may be meaningful to people living with chronic pain .

In addition to investigating the weather–pain relationship, we successfully conducted a national smartphone study that delivered on the promise of how consumer technology can support health research.[12,15] This study recruited over 10,000 participants throughout the United Kingdom, sustained daily self-reported data over many months,[13] and showcased the value of passively collected GPS data. Prior large smartphone studies have retained npj Digital Medicine (2019) 105

only around one in ten participants for seven days or less.[16,17] In contrast, our study retained 65% of participants for the first seven days, and 44% for the first month, with over 2600 participants contributing to the analysis having provided data for many months of the study.[13,14] An important success factor was strong public involvement in early setup and piloting, as well as participants’ interest in weather as a possible pain trigger.[14] The study design has resolved problems of prior weather–pain studies such as small populations,[5,7] short follow-up,[3,8] surrogate pain outcomes,[11] the absence of possible causal pathway variables such as mood, and assumptions about where participants were located and thus the weather to which they were exposed.[18,19]

Overcoming these obstacles produced a large dataset that allowed us to tease out subtle relationships between weather and pain .

There are potential limitations to this study. First, the reduction in participant numbers from over 10,000 with baseline data to the final 2658 participants with at least one within-month risk set raises questions about generalisability . Importantly, the characteristics of those included in the analysis were similar to the initial

10,000 participants, other than being slightly older (mean age 51 versus 48 years old). In a prior analysis, we showed that Cloudy participants were largely representative of a population reporting chronic-pain symptoms,[13] although proportionally fewer participants at both extremes of age were recruited. However, we would not expect middle-aged recruits to differ in their relationship between weather and pain from older or younger participants, and thus such selection factors would not invalidate our results. Second, the study was advertised to participants with a clear research question . It is possible that only people with a strong belief in a weather–pain relationship participated, generating an unrepresentative sample. However, the percentage of participants who believed in the weather –pain relationship was similar to prior studies,[20] and we did not see selective attrition of people who reported no weather–pain beliefs.[13] The within-person design would, regardless, mean that participants who drop out early would not introduce bias from time-invariant characteristics. Third, the lack of blinding raises possible information bias where observed weather could influence participants’ symptom reporting. Our baseline questionnaire demonstrated that rain and cold weather were the most common pre-existing beliefs. If a reporting bias were to exist, we would expect higher pain to be reported at times of colder weather . Our findings—including the absence of an association with either temperature or rainfall—cannot be explained by such a reporting bias. Fourth, pain reporting is subjective, meaning one participant’s “moderate” might equate to npj Digital Medicine (2019) 105

someone else’s “severe”. The within-person case-crossover analysis meant we compared moments when an individual’s score increased by a meaningful amount to a control period for that same person. Fifth, we chose to model the weather using daily averages. It is possible that other findings may be hidden if the association between weather and pain was with other metrics of weather , such as the daily maximum, minimum, or range, or even if the changes in weather on hourly time scales affect participants’ pain. Sixth, the findings from this United Kingdom study cannot necessarily be extrapolated to different climates where the weather is different . Seventh, our population-wide analysis assumed that all participants have the same weather –pain relationship. Different diseases may have different sensitivities to pain and, even within disease, participants may be affected differently. Our decision to use the whole chronic-pain population in our primary analysis means the overall associations with weather variables may be combinations of strong, weak and absent causal effects, thereby underestimating the most important associations. Notable differences were not seen after stratification by pain condition , although the power to detect any differences was reduced because of smaller sample sizes. Lastly, the inclusion of repeated events per person required us to consider within-subject dependence which, if not accounted for, would lead to bias.[21] Our outcome was based on changes in pain (a two or more category increase), which meant events rarely occurred on consecutive days , thereby ensuring a time gap between recurrent events and the avoidance of bias.

Understanding the relationship between weather and pain is important for several reasons . First, this study validates the perception of those who believe that their pain is associated with the weather . Second, given we can forecast the weather days in advance, understanding how weather relates to pain would allow pain forecasts . Patients could then plan activities and take greater control of their lives. Finally, understanding the relationship between weather and pain might also allow better understanding of the mechanisms for pain and thus allow the development of new and more effective interventions for those who suffer with pain .

In summary, our large national smartphone study has successfully supported the collection of daily symptoms and high-quality weather data , allowing examination of the relationship between weather and pain. The analysis has demonstrated significant relationships between relative humidity, pressure, wind speed and pain, with correlations remaining even when accounting for mood and physical activity .

Patient involvement has been important throughout the study , from inception to interpretation of the results. Co-author C.G. is a patient partner and co-applicant, while a patient and public involvement group of seven additional members has supported the study, meeting eight times in total . During the feasibility study,[14] patients positively influenced the wording and display of questions within the app . C.G. and other members of the Patient and Public Involvement Group were involved in media broadcasts at study launch and subsequent public engagement activities, explaining why the research question was important to them and relevant to patients with long-term pain conditions.[22] They have supported the interpretation of findings and the development of dissemination plans for the results, ensuring the results reach study participants, patient organizations and the general public.

We recruited participants through local and national media (television, radio, and press) and social media from 20 January 2016 to 20 January 2017. To participate in the study, participants needed to (i) be living with long-term (>3 months) pain conditions, (ii) be aged 17 years or older, (iii) be living in the United Kingdom, and (iv) own an Android or Apple iOS smartphone. Interested participants were directed to the study website (www.cloudywithachanceofpain.com) where they could check their eligibility, learn about the study, and download the uMotif app (Fig. 1). After downloading the study app, participants completed an electronic consent form and a baseline questionnaire including demographic information (sex, year of birth, first half of postcode), anatomical site(s) of pain, underlying pain condition(s), baseline medication use, and beliefs about the extent to which weather influenced their pain on a scale of 0–10, including which weather condition(s) were thought to be most associated with pain. Participants were then invited to collect daily symptoms for six months, or longer if willing. Each day, the app alerted participants to complete ten items at 6:24 p.m. (Fig. 1). The ten items were pain severity, fatigue, morning stiffness, impact of pain, sleep quality, time spent outside, waking up feeling tired, physical activity , mood, and well-being. Each data

Univariable (single weather variable only) Odds ratio (95% CI)

Multivariable (all weather variables only) Odds ratio (95% CI)

Multivariable (weather plus activity and mood) Odds ratio (95% CI)

Temperature Per 1 °C Per 1 s.d. (4.8 °C) Relative humidity Per 10% Per 1 s.d. (8.6%) Pressure Per 10 mbar Per 1 s.d. (11.1 mbar) Wind speed Per 1 m s–1 Per 1 s.d. (2.1 m s–1) High activity Low mood

1.011 (0.995–1.027) 1.022 (0.990–1.056) 0.939 (0.881–1.002) 4.083 (3.824–4.360)

High activity—Top three categories: 30 min or more of light or strenuous activity per day, or less than 30 min of strenuous activity Low mood—Bottom three categories: ‘depressed’, ‘feeling low’ or ‘not very happy’ s.d. standard deviation

Distribution of weather variables: Temperature: range −4.9 to 25.9 °C, s.d. 4.8 °C Relative humidity: range 43.8–100%, s.d. 8.6% Pressure: range 966–1044.8 mbar, s.d. 11.1 mbar Wind speed: range 0–21.5 m s−1, s.d. 2.1 m s−1 item had five possible labeled ordinal responses. For example, in response to the question “How severe was your pain today?”, possible responses were “no pain”, “mild pain”, “moderate pain”, “severe pain” or “very severe pain”. The data were analysed using a case-crossover design where, for each participant, exposure during days with a pain event (“hazard periods”) were compared to “control periods” without a pain event in the same month.[23] Pain events were defined as a two-or-more category increase in pain from the preceding day, consistent with more stringent definitions of a clinically important difference[24] (Fig. 3). Data collection ended on 20 April 2017.

Cohort selection Participants were included in the final cohort for analysis if they fulfilled the following criteria: (1) downloaded the app; (2) provided consent; (3) completed the baseline questionnaire; and (4) contributed at least one pain event and matched control period in the same month (see below). During exploratory analysis, it was apparent that people reported higher pain levels in the first ten days following recruitment (perhaps due to calibration or regression to the mean). Therefore, the first ten days were excluded from the formal analysis. However, even if the first ten days were included, they had a negligible effect on the results (Supplementary Table 12 ).

The total person-days in study was calculated for each participant as the number of days between their first and last day of entering pain data. The number of person-days on which a pain score was entered was summed per participant, presented as a proportion of the total person-days in study, and averaged across the population. The geographical distribution of recruitment was described as the number of UK postcode areas represented (out of a maximum of 124).[25] The movement of participants during the study was described as the median number of weather stations associated with each participant during their data-collection period.

Ethical approval

Ethical approval was obtained from the University of Manchester Research Ethics Committee (ref: ethics/15522) and from the NHS IRAS (ref: 23/NW/

0716). Participants were required to provide electronic consent for study inclusion. Further details are available elsewhere.[13,14]

Weather data were obtained by linking hourly smartphone GPS data to the nearest of 154 possible United Kingdom Met Office weather stations. Where GPS data were missing, we used significant location imputation. (For details, see supplement). Local hourly weather data were obtained from the Integrated Surface Database ( ISD ) of NOAA (http://www.ncdc. noaa.gov/isd), which includes hourly observations from UK Met Office weather stations.

Given the latitude–longitude coordinates of a participant location, the haversine distance to every Met Office weather station was calculated. The nearest station to the given location was selected, conditional on the distance being less than 100 km and the station having four weather variables (temperature, pressure, wind speed , and dewpoint temperature) available at that time. If all stations with the required weather data exceeded the maximum distance (100 km), the location was left unlinked and the observation was excluded from the analysis.

The significant location imputation approach for handling missing hourly GPS data had three stages.[26] First, the participant’s observed location data were ordered by the frequency that the locations were visited. Second, the locations were spatially clustered using Hartigan’s Leader Algorithm[27] with a threshold of 0.5 km. Third, missing locations during weekdays were replaced by the centroid of the participant’s most visited cluster for weekdays and missing locations during weekends were replaced by centroid of the participant’s most visited cluster for weekends.

Recruitment and duration of follow-up were presented as a graph of cumulative recruitment and active participants, with participation ending at the last symptom entry. Retention in the study was also presented as a survival probability against time since recruitment, with participants censored when they were no longer eligible for follow-up. Eligible follow-up time ranged from 90 days (for those recruited on 20 January 2017) to 456 days (for those recruited on 20 January 2016). Engagement of npj Digital Medicine (2019) 105

mbar. The day associated with the lowest estimated odds of a pain event was when the temperature was 7 °C, relative humidity was 67%, wind speed 4.5 m s−1 and pressure 1030 mbar participants was further described through clustering of engagement states, which has been described in detail elsewhere.[13] Following recruitment, individuals were labeled as engaged if they reported any of the ten symptoms on a given day. A first-order hidden Markov model was used to estimate the levels of engagement of participants by assuming three latent engagement states: high, low, and disengaged. Clusters were defined according to different probabilities of transitioning between high engagement, low engagement and disengagement during the study. Retention of active participants was also presented stratified by engagement cluster, and in the subset of participants who contributed to the final analysis.

Days without pain events were only control periods if they were eligible to have a two-or-more category increase (i.e. the preceding day’s pain was lower than “severe”), thus fulfilling the exchangeability assumption for the case-crossover study design.[28] With this design, participants serve as their own control, eliminating confounding by time-invariant factors. Each month per participant with at least one hazard and one control period formed a risk set. Conditional logistic regression was used to estimate the odds ratio ( OR ) for a pain event for four state weather variables (temperature, relative humidity , pressure, and wind speed ). The condition logistic regression model was implemented with the assumption that the possible recurrent events (hazard periods) within a person are independent conditional on the subject-specific variables and other observed timevarying covariates. Further, we make sure that there is no overlap between case and control periods. Our assumption is reasonable given the time gap between subsequent events.

Each weather variable was included in univariable models and then all four were included in a multivariable analysis. Each weather variable was represented as a daily average per participant for the hazard or control period, with results presented as an OR for a pain event in response to a one-unit increase for temperature and wind speed (°C and meter per second, respectively) or a ten-unit increase for relative humidity and pressure (percentage points and millibar, respectively). Standardized odds ratios of each weather variable were also calculated. The relative importance of the four state weather variables was estimated by summing the Akaike weights.[29] In all models, the preceding day’s pain score was included as it influenced the likelihood of a pain event the following day. The model was expanded to include mood and physical activity on the day of interest, included as binary variables. Time spent outside was considered as a possible effect modifier by including an interaction term with temperature, relative humidity , and wind speed . A directed acyclic graph is included in the supplementary material (Supplementary Fig. 5).

Sensitivity analyses were conducted to examine the effect of precipitation, day of the week, possible lag between weather and pain, change in weather from the day before the hazard or control day, disease type, sites of pain (single versus multiple sites) and prior beliefs in the weather–pain relationship. Respecting patients’ perspectives, we decided our primary analysis would focus on the whole chronic-pain population and our analyses of disease-specific associations would be secondary. We also reran the analysis including the first 10 days.

Daily pain-event estimates Estimated odds ratio for a pain event per day compared to the average weather day were calculated using the following equation: n

Odds Ratio 1⁄4 exp; βTðtemperature À μTÞ þ βRH 10 relative humidity À μRH

ÁÉ pressure À μp þ βwsp wind speed À μwsp þ βP 10 where βT = coefficient for temperature from final βRH = coefficient for relative humidity , βwsp = coefficient for wind speed , βP = coefficient for pressure, and μT = mean temperature, μRH = mean relative humidity , μwsp = mean wind speed , and μp = mean pressure model, of the daily UK means over the study period.

The predicted odds ratios of a pain event, relative to the average weather day, were plotted for all days within our study period for each of the four state weather variables. Statistical analyses were performed using R 3.3.0.30

22. Cloudy with a Chance of Pain on BBC North West Tonight. https://www.youtube.

Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.

com/watch?v=YUdtKGr49GY. Accessed 14 Oct 2019 (2016). 23. Maclure, M. The case-crossover design: a method for studying transient effects on the risk of acute events. Am. J. Epidemiol. 133, 144–153 (1991).

24. Olsen, M. F. et al Pain relief that matters to patients: systematic review of DATA AVAILABILITY

The datasets generated and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

empirical studies assessing the minimum clinically important difference in acute pain. BMC Med. 15, 35. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-016-0775-3 (2017). 25. BPH postcodes. A brief guide to UK postcodes. https://www.bph-postcodes.co. uk/guidetopc.cgi (2018).

26. Isaacman, S., Becker, R., Martonosi, M., Rowland, J., & Varshavsky, A. Identifying

CODE AVAILABILITY important places in people’s lives from cellular network data sibren. Proc. of 9th Int. Conf. Pervasive Comput. 1–18 (2011).

Data management and analyses were performed in R 3.3.0. Code may be available on

27. Hartigan, J. A. Clustering Algorithms. (Wiley, New York, 1975).

reasonable request.

28. Mittleman, M. A. & Mostofsky, E. Exchangeability in the case-crossover design. Int.

J. Epidemiol. 43, 1645–1655 (2014).

Received: 22 May 2019; Accepted: 23 September 2019; 29. Burnham, K. P., Anderson, D. R. Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach. 2nd ed. (Springer-Verlag, New York, 2003).

30. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing, https://www.r-project.org (2018).

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We are grateful for the contributions of our patient and public involvement group throughout the study: Carolyn Gamble, Karen Staniland, Shanali Perara, Simon Stones, Rebecca Parris, Annmarie Lewis, Dorothy Slater and Susan Moore . The study app and website was provided by uMotif Limited (London, UK) . The unique flowerlike ‘motif’ symptom tracking interface is owned by uMotif Limited and protected through EU Design Registrations and a U.S Design Patent . We gratefully acknowledge the National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration/National Climatic Data Center Integrated Surface Database (https://www.ncdc.noaa.gov/isd) for providing the weather data used in this study. The study was funded by Versus Arthritis (new name for Arthritis Research UK) (grant reference 21225), with additional support from the Centre for Epidemiology (grants 21755 and 20380). A.G. and A.M.V.C. are the recipients of Medical Research Council U.K. grants (MR/M022625/1 and MR/R013349/ 1). H.L.P. is the recipient of the Ken Muirden Overseas Training Fellowship from the Arthritis Australia, an educational research grant funded by the Australian Rheumatology Association. A.B. is supported by a Medical Research Council doctoral training partnership (grant MR/N013751/1). T.H. is supported by the Alan Turing Institute and the Royal Society (grant INF/R2/180067). D.M.S. is partially supported by the Natural Environment Research Council U.K. (grants NE/I005234/1, NE/I026545/1, and NE/N003918/1). R.S. is partially supported by the Alan Turing Institute (grant EP/ N510129/1).

W.G.D. designed the study, acquired funding, supervised and participated in datacollection and content analysis, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. A.L.B., B.B.Y. and H.L.P. conducted the analysis. L.C. coordinated project management and participant support. A.G., T.E.L., A.V.M.C., M.M., R.S., T.H., M.L., D.M.S., J.C.S. and J. McB. contributed to analysis plans and supervised the analysis. B.H., B.J., J.A., C.G., C.S., D.M.S., J.C.S. and J.McB. contributed to study design. C.S. led qualitative research in the feasibility study and led patient and public involvement. All authors critically reviewed manuscript drafts and approved the final version of the manuscript. W.G.D. is responsible for the overall content as guarantor, and attests that all listed authors meet authorship criteria and that no others meeting the criteria have been omitted.

COMPETING INTERESTS

W.G.D. has received consultancy fees from Bayer Pharmaceuticals and Google, unrelated to this study . B.J. and B.H. are co-founders of uMotif . All other authors declare no competing interests .

Supplementary information is available for this paper at https://doi.org/10.1038/ s41746-019-0180-3.

Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to W.G.D.

Reprints and permission information is available at http://www.nature.com/ reprints npj Digital Medicine (2019) 105

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative

Patient involvement has been important throughout the study , from inception to interpretation of the results. Co-author C.G. is a patient partner and co-applicant, while a patient and public involvement group of seven additional members has supported the study, meeting eight times in total. During the feasibility study,[14] patients positively influenced the wording and display of questions within the app. C.G. and other members of the Patient and Public Involvement Group were involved in media broadcasts at study launch and subsequent public engagement activities, explaining why the research question was important to them and relevant to patients with long-term pain conditions.[22] They have supported the interpretation of findings and the development of dissemination plans for the results, ensuring the results reach study participants, patient organizations and the general public.

Distribution of weather variables: Temperature: range −4.9 to 25.9 °C, s.d. 4.8 °C Relative humidity : range 43.8–100%, s.d. 8.6% Pressure: range 966–1044.8 mbar, s.d. 11.1 mbar Wind speed : range 0–21.5 m s−1, s.d. 2.1 m s−1 item had five possible labeled ordinal responses. For example, in response to the question “How severe was your pain today?”, possible responses were “no pain”, “mild pain”, “moderate pain”, “severe pain” or “very severe pain”. The data were analysed using a case-crossover design where, for each participant, exposure during days with a pain event (“hazard periods”) were compared to “control periods” without a pain event in the same month.[23] Pain events were defined as a two-or-more category increase in pain from the preceding day, consistent with more stringent definitions of a clinically important difference[24] (Fig. 3). Data collection ended on 20 April 2017.

The study app was downloaded by 13,207 users over the 12month recruitment period (Figs 1 and 2a) with recruitment from all 124 UK postcode areas. A total of 10,584 participants had complete baseline information and at least one pain entry, with 6850 (65%) participants remaining in the study beyond their first week and 4692 (44%) beyond their first month (Fig. 2b). Further description of engagement clusters is provided in Supplementary Table 2 and Supplementary Figs 1–3. A total of 2658 participants had at least one hazard period matched to a control period in the same month (Fig. 3) and were included in the final analysis. There were 9695 hazard periods included in the analysis for the final 2658 participants, matched to 81,727 control periods in 6431 participant-months. A total of 1235 participants contributed one month, and the remaining 1423 participants contributed 2–15 months.

The multivariable case-crossover analysis including the four state weather variables demonstrated that an increase in relative humidity was associated with a higher odds of a pain event with an OR of 1.139 (95% confidence interval 1.099–1.181) per 10 percentage point increase, as was an increase in wind speed with an OR of 1.02 (1.005–1.035) per 1 m s−1 increase ( Table 1 ). The odds of a pain event was lower with an increase in atmospheric pressure with an OR of 0.962 (0.937–0.987) per 10-mbar increase. Temperature did not have a significant association with pain ( OR 0.996 (0.985–1.007) per 1 °C increase). The odds of a pain event was 12% higher per one standard deviation increase in relative humidity (9 percentage points) ( OR 1.119 (1.084–1.154), compared to 4% lower for pressure ( OR 0.958 (0.930–0.989) and 4% higher for wind speed ( OR 1.041 (1.010–1.073) (11 mbar and 2 m s−1, respectively). Of the four weather variables, relative humidity had the strongest association with pain , and temperature the least, evidenced by the estimated relative importance of the variables and their standardized odds ratios ( Table 1 , Supplementary Table 4 ). Similar effect sizes were seen when each variable was examined in univariable analyses . Precipitation was not associated with an increased odds of a pain event ( OR 0.996 (0.989–1.003) per 1 mm daily rainfall amount) (Supplementary Table 5 ). Exploratory analyses considered time spent outside by including an interaction term with temperature, relative humidity , and wind speed . Time spent outside did not have a significant interaction with relative humidity or wind speed , nor did it lead to significant associations for temperature when conducting analyses stratified by time spent outside (Supplementary Table 3 ). It thus was not included in the final model.

Other factors such as day of the week (Supplementary Table 6 ), lagged weather values (Supplementary Table 7 ) and changes in weather variables from the previous day were tested. Mondays, Thursdays, and Saturdays ( ORs 1.14, 1.14, and 1.29, respectively) had higher odds of pain compared to Sundays, but adjusting for the day of the week did not alter the effect of the four main weather variables . Except for relative humidity (1-day lag and 2day lag), no significant associations were observed between lagged weather variables and pain events . Including change in weather from yesterday showed a minor effect of changing relative humidity ( OR 1.005 (1.001–1.009) per 10 percentage point increase), whereas the effects of today’s relative humidity and pressure remained unchanged (Supplementary Table 8 ). Stratification by disease led to a loss of statistical power and largely inconclusive results, although relative humidity appeared to have a stronger association with pain in patients with osteoarthritis (Supplementary Table 9 , Supplementary Fig. 4). Stratification by the number of pain sites also showed no meaningful difference (Supplementary Table 10 ). After stratifying by participants’ prior beliefs about their weather–pain relationship, relative humidity remained associated with pain in all participants although the association with pressure was only seen in those with a strong prior belief (Supplementary Table 11 ).

There are potential limitations to this study. First, the reduction in participant numbers from over 10,000 with baseline data to the final 2658 participants with at least one within-month risk set raises questions about generalisability. Importantly, the characteristics of those included in the analysis were similar to the initial

someone else’s “severe”. The within-person case-crossover analysis meant we compared moments when an individual’s score increased by a meaningful amount to a control period for that same person. Fifth, we chose to model the weather using daily averages. It is possible that other findings may be hidden if the association between weather and pain was with other metrics of weather , such as the daily maximum, minimum, or range, or even if the changes in weather on hourly time scales affect participants’ pain. Sixth, the findings from this United Kingdom study cannot necessarily be extrapolated to different climates where the weather is different. Seventh, our population-wide analysis assumed that all participants have the same weather –pain relationship. Different diseases may have different sensitivities to pain and, even within disease, participants may be affected differently. Our decision to use the whole chronic-pain population in our primary analysis means the overall associations with weather variables may be combinations of strong, weak and absent causal effects, thereby underestimating the most important associations. Notable differences were not seen after stratification by pain condition , although the power to detect any differences was reduced because of smaller sample sizes. Lastly, the inclusion of repeated events per person required us to consider within-subject dependence which, if not accounted for, would lead to bias.[21] Our outcome was based on changes in pain (a two or more category increase), which meant events rarely occurred on consecutive days , thereby ensuring a time gap between recurrent events and the avoidance of bias.

The reduction in participant numbers from over 10,000 with baseline data to the final 2658 participants with at least one within-month risk set raises questions about generalisability. The characteristics of those included in the analysis were similar to the initial

W.G.D. has received consultancy fees from Bayer Pharmaceuticals and Google, unrelated to this study. B.J. and B.H. are co-founders of uMotif. All other authors declare no competing interests

The study was funded by Versus Arthritis (new name for Arthritis Research UK) (grant reference 21225), with additional support from the Centre for Epidemiology (grants 21755 and 20380)

A.G. and A.M.V.C. are the recipients of Medical Research Council U.K. grants (MR/M022625/1 and MR/R013349/ 1)

H.L.P. is the recipient of the Ken Muirden Overseas Training Fellowship from the Arthritis Australia, an educational research grant funded by the Australian Rheumatology Association

A.B. is supported by a Medical Research Council doctoral training partnership (grant MR/N013751/1)

T.H. is supported by the Alan Turing Institute and the Royal Society (grant INF/R2/180067)

D.M.S. is partially supported by the Natural Environment Research Council U.K. (grants NE/I005234/1, NE/I026545/1, and NE/N003918/1)

R.S. is partially supported by the Alan Turing Institute (grant EP/ N510129/1)

Data and code

Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article

The datasets generated and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request

Supplementary information is available for this paper at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41746-019-0180-3

Reprints and permission information is available at http://www.nature.com/reprints npj Digital Medicine (2019)[105]

Supplementary information is available for this paper at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41746-019-0180-3 .

Try Scholarcy’s Flashcard Generator today

Import your papers and chapters to generate interactive summary flashcards that highlight key information, give you the definitions of key concepts, take you straight to the cited sources, and even more:

  • Quickly see how a study compares to earlier research
  • Download your references to import into your favourite reference manager
  • Download your flashcard to import into your favourite knowledge management tool, including Roam, Obsidian and Notion

Flashcard Generator

Each flashcard contains:

Key methods and results

Important claims and findings highlighted

Direct links to the full text of cited sources

Key concepts and their definitions

Comparative Analysis – see how the paper compares to earlier studies

Flashcard Generator

Need more than just a summary?

Upgrade to Scholarcy Library and build a personal research library by turning your collections of papers, RSS feeds, and search results into interactive, summary flashcards that you can read, share and annotate on any device.

You can also export your flashcards to a range of formats including Excel , Word , RIS and Markdown to generate annotated bibliographies, knowledge graphs and more.

What People Are Saying

“Quick processing time, successfully summarized important points”
“It’s really good for case study analysis, thank you for this too”
“I love this website so much it has made my research a lot easier thanks!”
“The instant feedback I get from this tool is amazing”
“Thank you for making my life easier”
“I don’t think I could have attained my Master’s in Systems Thinking without this fantastic platform.”
“Scholarcy is a great tool and helped me immensely with my studies”

Learn anything faster with Flashcards

Studylib is the easiest way to learn faster and retain knowledge for longer. Powered by cognitive science research our spaced repetition platform make studying as efficient as humanly possible

Join 1161705 users

research paper flash card

Powerful, intelligent flashcards

Learn anything with studylib flashcards. Create your own flashcards with custom images and audio. Improve your learning process with our spaced repetition planner Flashcards explorer

How to Learn Faster and Remember More?

Turn your chrome startpage into learning dashboard, educational library.

Search within the largest community-driven educational library. Read, print and download millions of worksheets, labs, lesson plans and other for free

Study collections

Organize and share your educational materials in our powerfull study colections.Add documents, flashcards, video, liks and share it with your friends or students!

research paper flash card

Get access to flashcards, worksheets, lesson plans, collections and other

Suggest us how to improve studylib.

(For complaints, use another form )

Input it if you want to receive answer

  • Find Flashcards
  • Why It Works
  • Tutors & resellers
  • Content partnerships
  • Teachers & professors
  • Employee training

Brainscape's Knowledge Genome TM

  • Entrance Exams
  • Professional Certifications
  • Foreign Languages
  • Medical & Nursing
  • Humanities & Social Studies
  • Mathematics
  • Health & Fitness
  • Business & Finance
  • Technology & Engineering
  • Food & Beverage
  • Random Knowledge

See full index

Learn research methods, research methods and statistics, by: sammi allouni, psyc 304 research methods athabasca, by: dan goldman.

MED 106- RESEARCH METHODS AND ESSENTIAL MEDICAL STATISTICS

MED 106- RESEARCH METHODS AND ESSENTIAL MEDICAL STATISTICS

By: elena .m.

AQA Psychology - Research Methods

AQA Psychology - Research Methods

By: kj hall, research methods, by: sophie a.

Basic Research Method 23

Basic Research Method 23

By: helene biallas, research methods ibug99, by: axel eriksson, sociology - research methods, by: barney hennessey, psychology a2 research methods, by: elliott wymer.

Psychology - Research Methods

Psychology - Research Methods

By: ben tanner, psy1017- cognitive psychology with research methods 1, by: laura mantle.

Research Methods

By: Holly Tomlinson

By: jeffrey driscoll, lb - as psychology research methods, by: laura backhouse, psy1016 biological psychology with research methods 1, by: sarah assaf.

Research Methods

By: Beth Chambers

EPI Research Methods and Statistics

EPI Research Methods and Statistics

By: birwe leon, psych (1.6) research methods, by: tommy radford.

A-Level OCR Psychology- Research Methods

A-Level OCR Psychology- Research Methods

By: sian sanghera, as/a2 research methods, by: bethan dance, by: kimberley küster, research methods and statistics in psychology, psychology research methods and statistics, by: erika ruhnke.

Research Methods 1

Research Methods 1

By: grace herring.

PR3144 Principles of Research Methods

PR3144 Principles of Research Methods

By: matthew tan, y1: sociology: research methods, by: nikita hennessey, zpsychology research methods, by: 0a548ck kan3, sem 2 research methods & stats.

AQA A-Level Psychology Research Methods

AQA A-Level Psychology Research Methods

Knowledge genome.

  • Corporate Training
  • Teachers & Schools
  • Android App
  • Help Center
  • Law Education
  • All Subjects A-Z
  • All Certified Classes
  • Earn Money!

show password

Make sure to remember your password. If you forget it there is no way for StudyStack to send you a reset link. You would need to create a new account. Your email address is only used to allow you to reset your password. See our Privacy Policy and Terms of Service .

Already a StudyStack user? Log In

research paper

QuestionAnswer
How long did the medieval time period last ? from 500 to 1500 AD
What is the difference between the Middle Ages and Medieval times ? no difference
What is a feudal system ? When local lords grant land for service
Who ran the Manor ? ran by a lord who lived in a big house
Who signed the Magna Carta ? king John
When was the Magna Carta signed ? 1215 AD
Where did the black death likely start at ? in Asia traveled towards the silk road
What kind of climate does Europe have ? hot summers and wet winters
What's a pope ? head of the church
Why is it important to know about the Middle Ages ? it's a big part of not only European history but world history

Use these flashcards to help memorize information. Look at the large card and try to recall what is on the other side. Then click the card to flip it. If you knew the answer, click the green Know box. Otherwise, click the red Don't know box.

When you've placed seven or more cards in the Don't know box, click "retry" to try those cards again.

If you've accidentally put the card in the wrong box, just click on the card to take it out of the box.

You can also use your keyboard to move the cards as follows:

  • SPACEBAR - flip the current card
  • LEFT ARROW - move card to the Don't know pile
  • RIGHT ARROW - move card to Know pile
  • BACKSPACE - undo the previous action

If you are logged in to your account, this website will remember which cards you know and don't know so that they are in the same box the next time you log in.

When you need a break, try one of the other activities listed below the flashcards like Matching, Snowman, or Hungry Bug. Although it may feel like you're playing a game, your brain is still making more connections with the information to help you out.

To see how well you know the information, try the Quiz or Test activity.

"Know" box contains:
Time elapsed:
Retries:

Finish Sign Up

Continue before upload has complete.

Only the currently loaded and selected files will be processed. You have 0 pages selected and there are 0 remaining to be uploaded.

All pages uploaded. Click confirm to proceed.

Flashcard Type

Front text length, back text length.

Please login or sign up to save your options.

AI Flashcard Generator

Preparing for a test, exam, or quiz? Revisely's flashcard maker automatically transforms your pictures, notes, PDFs, PowerPoints and other documents into flashcards using the power of AI. Simply upload your materials, and our AI will create your flashcards in seconds.

Step 1

Step 1: Upload

Upload an image, take a picture, or provide a PDF, and let our AI make flashcards for you. It's free with no account required.

Step 2

Step 2: Practice

Review your flashcards, share, print, or export them. We support platforms like Anki and many others.

Step 3

Step 3: Test Yourself

Once you've mastered your flashcards, enter exam mode. Test your knowledge and receive personalized feedback, generated by AI.

Creating printable flashcards has never been easier. Choose single-sided for a quick cut and stick approach or go with double-sided, which comes auto-aligned for your convenience.

Works in over 50 languages including Russian 🇷🇺

Try it out by uploading a screenshot, photo, or simply inputting your text content below. It even works with handwritten content.

Upload

Drag a document here or click to browse

Subscribe to Generate Unlimited AI Flashcards

Unlock the full potential of your study materials with our subscription plans. Whether it's your notes or a textbook, our AI intelligently crafts flashcards that make studying more impactful and less time-consuming.

No subscription required

  • Unlimited non-AI Flashcards
  • 5 pages per document
  • 20 exam mode answers
  • 20,000 characters per text upload
  • No exporting
  • Limited new features

Billed annually

  • Unlimited AI Flashcards
  • 100 pages per document
  • Unlimited exam mode answers
  • 60,000 characters per text upload
  • Export to Anki, PDF and more
  • New features soon

Billed monthly

Optional Upgrade

You have selected the AI Flashcard Generator . Upgrade to include unlimited access to all of our AI tools for just more per month.

AI Flashcard Generator Only

All generators.

Already subscribed? Login to an existing account.

Flashcard Machine - create, study and share online flash cards

My flashcards, flashcard library.

Create Account

  • Flashcards >>
  • Research Paper

Shared Flashcard Set

.

.

Cards Return to Set Details

Term
never explains itself. It is the first person narrative of Esther's perceptions. It's a slice of her life. It's a vivid picture of the inside of a mind through insanity as well as the precursor and the healing."
  • Collaborative Sets
  • Study Sessions
  • Flashcard Pages
  • About FlashcardMachine
  • Support Form
  • Privacy Policy
  • Terms of Use
  • Getting Started
  • Apple App Store
  • Google Play
  • Amazon Apps

project

Our Advantages

check

Secure Payments

cards

For security reasons we do not store any credit card information.

A complete guide to research papers writing

Research the topic, research the sources, organise your information, craft the outline, compose the introduction, compose the body, compose the conclusion, how not to write a research paper.

A research paper is an academic paper where you provide the results of your own investigation regarding a specific topic. It can be a master’s paper or even a dissertation. Your knowledge is not enough to craft an A+ paper, you will need to read and analyze dozens of reliable resources. This task may make you spend long sleepless nights over the project. We know how hard it can be to cope with such a complicated writing task, so we’re always ready to assist you

We are a team of experienced writers. We have written over a hundred of research papers for students from different countries. Placing an order will take a few minutes of your time and it will save you from hard work. Here’s the variant of how we can provide you research paper writing services:

  • You place an order on the website and indicate all the requirements;
  • We find you a research paper writer that will fit your requirements;
  • The writer does the research and writes the text of the paper;
  • You get the research paper in time and offer some edits if needed.

Get a high-quality text from the best research paper writing services.

How to write a research paper: a step-by-step guide

In this section of the page, we’ll give you a complete guide to write a good research paper. You will see how to craft the paper from scratch. Follow the guide from the first step to the last one or pick up only the steps that you need for the paper.

Many students treat this stage of research papers writing as the easiest one and they face a lot of difficulties when doing the research in the future. Your goal here is to find the topic that hasn’t been discovered before, but the one that will give you the chance to find enough reliable sources. Your topic should be up-to-date and it should be interesting for your readers. It’s a big misconception to think that no one will ever read your paper. The right topic will make people look inside.

A research paper writer always does the research when crafting a paper

Start your research with crafting a list of primary and secondary sources. The first list include books, articles from scientists or famous researchers and all the recommendations from your professor. The second list will include the rest of sources that are somehow related to the topic, but you cannot fully rely on them. It means you can use information for analyzing the topic from various points of view to come to your own conclusion.

There are three major ways of how you can keep all the information in order:

  • Taking notes;
  • Prioritizing your information from the most to the least relevant;
  • Making bibliography cards.

If you don’t try to put everything in order, you will have to start your research somewhere from the middle as you won’t be able to keep in mind so much data. You can use a pen and your notebook or you can use any software for planning or word processing.

When you have everything in the right order, it’s time to plan your paper and think how it will look like. An outline is a detailed plan where you will add quotations, some core statements and the thesis. If you order only an outline from the research paper writing service, you will easily craft the text as you will get the background information that you will complete. Your outline should represent the structure of your paper with as much detail as possible.

Read the tips to write a research paper with ease.

Start your paper with writing how important the topic of your research is. Give your readers a few reasons why they should view your paper. Write a few sentences about prior researches that you have found during the research stage. And finish the introduction with the thesis statement. Express the core idea of the paper and your position towards it. Don’t make any hints at the conclusions as your readers will lose their interest.

Use the outline and write each section of the body separately. We recommend you using a ladder of abstraction technique. It means to start with general information and finish with some specific facts. You can do it within one section. To make your text readable, divide it into paragraphs. Use transitions between them to make them logically connected. When you finish, read the text and think if it’s easy to understand.

Make a summary of the thesis statement and all the arguments that you have provided in the body part of the paper. Professional research paper writers always write one phrase about the significance of the research to remind readers about it. You can finish your paper in a traditional way by stating of there’s anything for the further research. Or you can finish your conclusion with a question to your readers about the need to do any researches in the future. It’s good to discuss it with your professor.

Research papers writing is easy thanks to a complete guide on this page.

Most of the students make the same mistakes. The first is presence of grammar errors or misspelling. You can do a super great research and provide fantastic solutions to some problems, but you will spoil the overall impression. The next is procrastination. Most of the students are sure that they can manage cope with the task in one night before the deadline. And one more common mistake is using slangs and jargons. It’s an academic piece of writing, so mind you style.

  • Research Guides
  • Vanderbilt University Libraries

Artificial Intelligence for Peabody College

Ai research tools.

  • AI @ Vanderbilt
  • AI & Academic Integrity
  • AI & Ethics
  • AI Citation and Attribution

About This Table

The resources described in the table represent an incomplete list of tools specifically geared toward exploring and synthesizing research. As generative AI becomes more integrated into  online   search tools , even the early research and topic development stages could incorporate AI. If you have any questions about using these tools for your research, please email us at [email protected]

Vanderbilt’s new private ChatGPT platform. It is a writing tool, idea generator, and code generator.

It is locally hosted, so data can be uploaded without the purported risk of becoming public. Use caution.

 

Free for VU faculty and staff. Requires VU credentials. Use the Claude, ChatGPT, and Mistral LLMs through this interface.
While the AI chatbot ChatGPT is typically considered a writing tool, it can also be used in the initial idea development phase of research to help find further sources. (Remember to always look up sources to verify their credibility.) The  . The free version was trained on data last updated in September 2021, but that might change. There is a free version available.
Like Research Rabbit, Connected Papers focuses on the relationships between research papers to find similar research. You can also use Connected Papers to overview an academic field visually. Semantic Scholar Database Free (5 graphs/month); paid version allows unlimited graphing.
Like Elicit, Consensus uses LLMs to help researchers find and synthesize answers to research questions, focusing on each paper's scholarly authors' findings and claims. Semantic Scholar Database Free (20 searches/month); Paid version allows unlimited searching.
Using large language models (LLMs), Elicit finds papers relevant to your topic by searching through papers and citations and extracting and synthesizing key information. Semantic Scholar Database Free trial available. Pay for credits after the trial expires.
Google designed Gemini (formerly Bard), an AI-powered chatbot that responds to natural language queries with relevant information. As with ChatGPT, researchers can use Gemini to aid in topic development and initial source discovery. Gemini can currently connect to the Internet. Gemini is currently free to use. A personal Google account is required and does not work with VU accounts.
Using LLMs, Perplexity is a search engine that provides AI-generated answers (much like ChatGPT), including citations linked above the summaries. Internal search index Free with paid subscriptions available.
Research Rabbit is a citation-based mapping tool that focuses on the relationships between research works. It uses visualizations to help researchers find similar papers to those of other researchers. Research Rabbit uses multiple databases but does not name them (more information can be found on the  ). Research Rabbit is currently free.
Scholarcy summarizes key points and claims of articles into 'summary cards' that researchers can read, share, and annotate when compiling research on a given topic. Scholarcy only uses  . It helps you read and summarize your research but is not a search engine. Free (short articles only); Paid version allows articles of any length.
scite has a suite of products that help researchers develop their topics, find papers, and search citations in context (describing whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence)  Many different sources (an incomplete list can be found  ). No. ( ) ;  .
Semantic Scholar (which supplies underlying data for many of the other tools on this list) provides summaries (TLDRs) of papers' main objectives and results. Semantic Scholar Database Semantic Scholar is currently free.
  • << Previous: AI & Academic Integrity
  • Next: AI & Ethics >>
  • Last Updated: Aug 28, 2024 2:31 PM
  • URL: https://researchguides.library.vanderbilt.edu/aiforpeabody

Creative Commons License

Cart

  • SUGGESTED TOPICS
  • The Magazine
  • Newsletters
  • Managing Yourself
  • Managing Teams
  • Work-life Balance
  • The Big Idea
  • Data & Visuals
  • Reading Lists
  • Case Selections
  • HBR Learning
  • Topic Feeds
  • Account Settings
  • Email Preferences

Research: How to Build Consensus Around a New Idea

  • Devon Proudfoot
  • Wayne Johnson

research paper flash card

Strategies for overcoming the disagreements that can stymie innovation.

Previous research has found that new ideas are seen as risky and are often rejected. New research suggests that this rejection can be due to people’s lack of shared criteria or reference points when evaluating a potential innovation’s value. In a new paper, the authors find that the more novel the idea, the more people differ on their perception of its value. They also found that disagreement itself can make people view ideas as risky and make them less likely to support them, regardless of how novel the idea is. To help teams get on the same page when it comes to new ideas, they suggest gathering information about evaluator’s reference points and developing criteria that can lead to more focused discussions.

Picture yourself in a meeting where a new idea has just been pitched, representing a major departure from your company’s standard practices. The presenter is confident about moving forward, but their voice is quickly overtaken by a cacophony of opinions from firm opposition to enthusiastic support. How can you make sense of the noise? What weight do you give each of these opinions? And what does this disagreement say about the idea?

research paper flash card

  • DP Devon Proudfoot is an Associate Professor of Human Resource Studies at Cornell’s ILR School. She studies topics related to diversity and creativity at work.
  • Wayne Johnson is a researcher at the Utah Eccles School of Business. He focuses on evaluations and decisions about new information, including persuasion regarding creative ideas and belief change.

Partner Center

Suggestions or feedback?

MIT News | Massachusetts Institute of Technology

  • Machine learning
  • Sustainability
  • Black holes
  • Classes and programs

Departments

  • Aeronautics and Astronautics
  • Brain and Cognitive Sciences
  • Architecture
  • Political Science
  • Mechanical Engineering

Centers, Labs, & Programs

  • Abdul Latif Jameel Poverty Action Lab (J-PAL)
  • Picower Institute for Learning and Memory
  • Lincoln Laboratory
  • School of Architecture + Planning
  • School of Engineering
  • School of Humanities, Arts, and Social Sciences
  • Sloan School of Management
  • School of Science
  • MIT Schwarzman College of Computing

Study reveals the benefits and downside of fasting

Press contact :, media download.

A large glowing stem cell, with clocks and empty plates in background.

*Terms of Use:

Images for download on the MIT News office website are made available to non-commercial entities, press and the general public under a Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial No Derivatives license . You may not alter the images provided, other than to crop them to size. A credit line must be used when reproducing images; if one is not provided below, credit the images to "MIT."

A large glowing stem cell, with clocks and empty plates in background.

Previous image Next image

Low-calorie diets and intermittent fasting have been shown to have numerous health benefits: They can delay the onset of some age-related diseases and lengthen lifespan, not only in humans but many other organisms.

Many complex mechanisms underlie this phenomenon. Previous work from MIT has shown that one way fasting exerts its beneficial effects is by boosting the regenerative abilities of intestinal stem cells, which helps the intestine recover from injuries or inflammation.

In a study of mice, MIT researchers have now identified the pathway that enables this enhanced regeneration, which is activated once the mice begin “refeeding” after the fast. They also found a downside to this regeneration: When cancerous mutations occurred during the regenerative period, the mice were more likely to develop early-stage intestinal tumors.

“Having more stem cell activity is good for regeneration, but too much of a good thing over time can have less favorable consequences,” says Omer Yilmaz, an MIT associate professor of biology, a member of MIT’s Koch Institute for Integrative Cancer Research, and the senior author of the new study.

Yilmaz adds that further studies are needed before forming any conclusion as to whether fasting has a similar effect in humans.

“We still have a lot to learn, but it is interesting that being in either the state of fasting or refeeding when exposure to mutagen occurs can have a profound impact on the likelihood of developing a cancer in these well-defined mouse models,” he says.

MIT postdocs Shinya Imada and Saleh Khawaled are the lead authors of the paper, which appears today in Nature .

Driving regeneration

For several years, Yilmaz’s lab has been investigating how fasting and low-calorie diets affect intestinal health. In a 2018 study , his team reported that during a fast, intestinal stem cells begin to use lipids as an energy source, instead of carbohydrates. They also showed that fasting led to a significant boost in stem cells’ regenerative ability.

However, unanswered questions remained: How does fasting trigger this boost in regenerative ability, and when does the regeneration begin?

“Since that paper, we’ve really been focused on understanding what is it about fasting that drives regeneration,” Yilmaz says. “Is it fasting itself that’s driving regeneration, or eating after the fast?”

In their new study, the researchers found that stem cell regeneration is suppressed during fasting but then surges during the refeeding period. The researchers followed three groups of mice — one that fasted for 24 hours, another one that fasted for 24 hours and then was allowed to eat whatever they wanted during a 24-hour refeeding period, and a control group that ate whatever they wanted throughout the experiment.

The researchers analyzed intestinal stem cells’ ability to proliferate at different time points and found that the stem cells showed the highest levels of proliferation at the end of the 24-hour refeeding period. These cells were also more proliferative than intestinal stem cells from mice that had not fasted at all.

“We think that fasting and refeeding represent two distinct states,” Imada says. “In the fasted state, the ability of cells to use lipids and fatty acids as an energy source enables them to survive when nutrients are low. And then it’s the postfast refeeding state that really drives the regeneration. When nutrients become available, these stem cells and progenitor cells activate programs that enable them to build cellular mass and repopulate the intestinal lining.”

Further studies revealed that these cells activate a cellular signaling pathway known as mTOR, which is involved in cell growth and metabolism. One of mTOR’s roles is to regulate the translation of messenger RNA into protein, so when it’s activated, cells produce more protein. This protein synthesis is essential for stem cells to proliferate.

The researchers showed that mTOR activation in these stem cells also led to production of large quantities of polyamines — small molecules that help cells to grow and divide.

“In the refed state, you’ve got more proliferation, and you need to build cellular mass. That requires more protein, to build new cells, and those stem cells go on to build more differentiated cells or specialized intestinal cell types that line the intestine,” Khawaled says.

Too much of a good thing

The researchers also found that when stem cells are in this highly regenerative state, they are more prone to become cancerous. Intestinal stem cells are among the most actively dividing cells in the body, as they help the lining of the intestine completely turn over every five to 10 days. Because they divide so frequently, these stem cells are the most common source of precancerous cells in the intestine.

In this study, the researchers discovered that if they turned on a cancer-causing gene in the mice during the refeeding stage, they were much more likely to develop precancerous polyps than if the gene was turned on during the fasting state. Cancer-linked mutations that occurred during the refeeding state were also much more likely to produce polyps than mutations that occurred in mice that did not undergo the cycle of fasting and refeeding.

“I want to emphasize that this was all done in mice, using very well-defined cancer mutations. In humans it’s going to be a much more complex state,” Yilmaz says. “But it does lead us to the following notion: Fasting is very healthy, but if you’re unlucky and you’re refeeding after a fasting, and you get exposed to a mutagen, like a charred steak or something, you might actually be increasing your chances of developing a lesion that can go on to give rise to cancer.”

Yilmaz also noted that the regenerative benefits of fasting could be significant for people who undergo radiation treatment, which can damage the intestinal lining, or other types of intestinal injury. His lab is now studying whether polyamine supplements could help to stimulate this kind of regeneration, without the need to fast.

“This fascinating study provides insights into the complex interplay between food consumption, stem cell biology, and cancer risk,” says Ophir Klein, a professor of medicine at the University of California at San Francisco and Cedars-Sinai Medical Center, who was not involved in the study. “Their work lays a foundation for testing polyamines as compounds that may augment intestinal repair after injuries, and it suggests that careful consideration is needed when planning diet-based strategies for regeneration to avoid increasing cancer risk.”

The research was funded, in part, by a Pew-Stewart Trust Scholar award, the Marble Center for Cancer Nanomedicine, the Koch Institute-Dana Farber/Harvard Cancer Center Bridge Project, and the MIT Stem Cell Initiative.

Share this news article on:

Press mentions, medical news today.

A new study led by researchers at MIT suggests that fasting and then refeeding stimulates cell regeneration in the intestines, reports Katharine Lang for Medical News Today . However, notes Lang, researchers also found that fasting “carries the risk of stimulating the formation of intestinal tumors.” 

Prof. Ömer Yilmaz and his colleagues have discovered the potential health benefits and consequences of fasting, reports Max Kozlov for Nature . “There is so much emphasis on fasting and how long to be fasting that we’ve kind of overlooked this whole other side of the equation: what is going on in the refed state,” says Yilmaz.

MIT researchers have discovered how fasting impacts the regenerative abilities of intestinal stem cells, reports Ed Cara for Gizmodo . “The major finding of our current study is that refeeding after fasting is a distinct state from fasting itself,” explain Prof. Ömer Yilmaz and postdocs Shinya Imada and Saleh Khawaled. “Post-fasting refeeding augments the ability of intestinal stem cells to, for example, repair the intestine after injury.” 

Previous item Next item

Related Links

  • Omer Yilmaz
  • Koch Institute
  • Department of Biology

Related Topics

Related articles.

On dark background is a snake-like shape of colorful tumor cells, mainly in blue. Near top are pinkish-red cells, and near bottom are lime-green cells.

How early-stage cancer cells hide from the immune system

MIT biologists found that intestinal stem cells express high levels of a ketogenic enzyme called HMGCS2, shown in brown.

Study links certain metabolites to stem cell function in the intestine

Intestinal stem cells from mice that fasted for 24 hours, at right, produced much more substantial intestinal organoids than stem cells from mice that did not fast, at left.

Fasting boosts stem cells’ regenerative capacity

“Not only does the high-fat diet change the biology of stem cells, it also changes the biology of non-stem-cell populations, which collectively leads to an increase in tumor formation,” Omer Yilmaz says.

How diet influences colon cancer

More mit news.

Illustration of 5 spheres with purple and brown swirls. Below that, a white koala with insets showing just its head. Each koala has one purple point on either the forehead, ears, and nose.

A framework for solving parabolic partial differential equations

Read full story →

Feyisayo Eweje wears lab coat and gloves while sitting in a lab.

Designing better delivery for medical therapies

Saeed Miganeh poses standing in a hallway. A street scene is visible through windows in the background

Making a measurable economic impact

Jessica Tam headshot

Faces of MIT: Jessica Tam

More than a dozen people sit around shared tables with laptops running App Inventor

First AI + Education Summit is an international push for “AI fluency”

Sally Kornbluth speaking at a podium

President Kornbluth welcomes the Class of 2028

  • More news on MIT News homepage →

Massachusetts Institute of Technology 77 Massachusetts Avenue, Cambridge, MA, USA

  • Map (opens in new window)
  • Events (opens in new window)
  • People (opens in new window)
  • Careers (opens in new window)
  • Accessibility
  • Social Media Hub
  • MIT on Facebook
  • MIT on YouTube
  • MIT on Instagram
  • Artificial Intelligence

Exclusive: New Research Finds Stark Global Divide in Ownership of Powerful AI Chips

The World AI Conference in Shanghai

W hen we think of the “cloud,” we often imagine data floating invisibly in the ether. But the reality is far more tangible: the cloud is located in huge buildings called data centers, filled with powerful, energy-hungry computer chips. Those chips, particularly graphics processing units (GPUs), have become a critical piece of infrastructure for the world of AI, as they are required to build and run powerful chatbots like ChatGPT.

As the number of things you can do with AI grows, so does the geopolitical importance of high-end chips—and where they are located in the world. The U.S. and China are competing to amass stockpiles, with Washington enacting sanctions aimed at preventing Beijing from buying the most cutting-edge varieties. But despite the stakes, there is a surprising lack of public data on where exactly the world’s AI chips are located.

A new peer-reviewed paper , shared exclusively with TIME ahead of its publication, aims to fill that gap. “We set out to find: Where is AI?” says Vili Lehdonvirta, the lead author of the paper and a professor at Oxford University’s Internet Institute. Their findings were stark: GPUs are highly concentrated in only 30 countries in the world, with the U.S. and China far out ahead. Much of the world lies in what the authors call “Compute Deserts:” areas where there are no GPUs for hire at all.

The finding has significant implications not only for the next generation of geopolitical competition, but for AI governance—or, which governments have the power to regulate how AI is built and deployed. “If the actual infrastructure that runs the AI, or on which the AI is trained, is on your territory, then you can enforce compliance,” says Lehdonvirta, who is also a professor of technology policy at Aalto University. Countries without jurisdiction over AI infrastructure have fewer legislative choices, he argues, leaving them subjected to a world shaped by others. “This has implications for which countries shape AI development as well as norms around what is good, safe, and beneficial AI,” says Boxi Wu, one of the paper’s authors.

The paper maps the physical locations of “public cloud GPU compute”—essentially, GPU clusters that are accessible for hire via the cloud businesses of major tech companies. But the research has some big limitations: it doesn’t count GPUs that are held by governments, for example, or in the private hands of tech companies for their use alone. And it doesn’t factor in non-GPU varieties of chips that are increasingly being used to train and run advanced AI. Lastly, it doesn't count individual chips, but rather the number of compute “regions” (or groups of data centers containing those chips) that cloud businesses make available in each country.

Read More: How ‘Friendshoring’ Made Southeast Asia Pivotal to the AI Revolution

That’s not for want of trying. “GPU quantities and especially how they are distributed across [cloud] providers’ regions,” the paper notes, “are treated as highly confidential information.” Even with the paper’s limitations, its authors argue, the research is the closest up-to-date public estimate of where in the world the most advanced AI chips are located—and a good proxy for the elusive bigger picture.

The paper finds that the U.S. and China have by far the most public GPU clusters in the world. China leads the U.S. on the number of GPU-enabled regions overall, however the most advanced GPUs are highly concentrated in the United States. The U.S. has eight “regions” where H100 GPUs—the kind that are the subject of U.S. government sanctions on China—are available to hire. China has none. This does not mean that China has no H100s; it only means that cloud companies say they do not have any H100 GPUs located in China. There is a burgeoning black market in China for the restricted chips, the New York Times reported in August, citing intelligence officials and vendors who said that many millions of dollars worth of chips had been smuggled into China despite the sanctions.

research paper flash card

The paper’s authors argue that the world can be divided into three categories: “Compute North,” where the most advanced chips are located; the “Compute South,” which has some older chips suited for running, but not training, AI systems; and “Compute Deserts,” where no chips are available for hire at all. The terms—which overlap to an extent with the fuzzy “Global North” and “Global South” concepts used by some development economists—are just an analogy intended to draw attention to the “global divisions” in AI compute, Lehdonvirta says. 

The risk of chips being so concentrated in rich economies, says Wu, is that countries in the global south may become reliant on AIs developed in the global north without having a say in how they work. 

It “mirrors existing patterns of global inequalities across the so-called Global North and South,” Wu says, and threatens to “entrench the economic, political and technological power of Compute North countries, with implications for Compute South countries’ agency in shaping AI research and development.”

More Must-Reads from TIME

  • Breaking Down the 2024 Election Calendar
  • What if Ultra-Processed Foods Aren’t as Bad as You Think?
  • How Ukraine Beat Russia in the Battle of the Black Sea
  • The Reintroduction of Kamala Harris
  • Long COVID Looks Different in Kids
  • How Project 2025 Would Jeopardize Americans’ Health
  • What a $129 Frying Pan Says About America’s Eating Habits
  • The 32 Most Anticipated Books of Fall 2024

Write to Billy Perrigo at [email protected]

  • Skip to main content
  • Keyboard shortcuts for audio player

Research News

The physics behind the most annoying thing that could ever happen to you: a paper cut, the physics behind a very annoying thing that could ever happen to you: a paper cut.

Scientists have figured out what type of paper is the most prone to cut skin. Kaare Jensen, associate professor of physics at the Technical University of Denmark, explains.

Copyright © 2024 NPR. All rights reserved. Visit our website terms of use and permissions pages at www.npr.org for further information.

NPR transcripts are created on a rush deadline by an NPR contractor. This text may not be in its final form and may be updated or revised in the future. Accuracy and availability may vary. The authoritative record of NPR’s programming is the audio record.

Research shows our bodies go through rapid changes in our 40s and our 60s

For many people, reaching their mid-40s may bring unpleasant signs the body isn’t working as well as it once did. Injuries seem to happen more frequently. Muscles may feel weaker.

A new study, published Wednesday in Nature Aging , shows what may be causing the physical decline. Researchers have found that molecules and microorganisms both inside and outside our bodies are going through dramatic changes, first at about age 44 and then again when we hit 60. Those alterations may be causing significant differences in cardiovascular health and immune function.

The findings come from Stanford scientists who analyzed blood and other biological samples of 108 volunteers ages 25 to 75, who continued to donate samples for several years. 

“While it’s obvious that you’re aging throughout your entire life, there are two big periods where things really shift,” said the study’s senior author, Michael Snyder, a professor of genetics and director of the Center for Genomics and Personalized Medicine at Stanford Medicine. For example, “there’s a big shift in the metabolism of lipids when people are in their 40s and in the metabolism of carbohydrates when people are in their 60s.”

Lipids are fatty substances, including LDL, HDL and triglycerides, that perform a host of functions in the body, but they can be harmful if they build up in the blood.

The scientists tracked many kinds of molecules in the samples, including RNA and proteins, as well as the participants’ microbiomes.

The metabolic changes the researchers discovered indicate not that people in their 40s are burning calories more slowly but rather that the body is breaking food down differently. The scientists aren’t sure exactly what impact those changes have on health.

Previous research showed that resting energy use, or metabolic rate , didn’t change from ages 20 to 60. The new study’s findings don't contradict that.

The changes in metabolism affect how the body reacts to alcohol or caffeine, although the health consequences aren’t yet clear. In the case of caffeine, it may result in higher sensitivity. 

It’s also not known yet whether the shifts could be linked to lifestyle or behavioral factors. For example, the changes in alcohol metabolism might be because people are drinking more in their mid-40s, Snyder said.

For now, Snyder suggests people in their 40s keep a close eye on their lipids, especially LDL cholesterol.

“If they start going up, people might want to think about taking statins if that’s what their doctor recommends,” he said. Moreover, “knowing there’s a shift in the molecules that affect muscles and skin, you might want to warm up more before exercising so you don’t hurt yourself.”

Until we know better what those changes mean, the best way to deal with them would be to eat healthy foods and to exercise regularly, Snyder said.Dr. Josef Coresh, founding director of the Optimal Aging Institute at the NYU Grossman School of Medicine, compared the new findings to the invention of the microscope.

“The beauty of this type of paper is the level of detail we can see in molecular changes,” said Coresh, a professor of medicine at the school. “But it will take time to sort out what individual changes mean and how we can tailor medications to those changes. We do know that the origins of many diseases happen in midlife when people are in their 40s, though the disease may occur decades later.”

The new study “is an important step forward,” said Dr. Lori Zeltser, a professor of pathology and cell biology at the Columbia University Vagelos College of Physicians and Surgeons. While we don’t know what the consequences of those metabolic changes are yet, “right now, we have to acknowledge that we metabolize food differently in our 40s, and that is something really new.”

The shifts the researchers found might help explain numerous age-related health changes, such as muscle loss, because “your body is breaking down food differently,” Zeltser said.

Linda Carroll is a regular health contributor to NBC News. She is coauthor of "The Concussion Crisis: Anatomy of a Silent Epidemic" and "Out of the Clouds: The Unlikely Horseman and the Unwanted Colt Who Conquered the Sport of Kings." 

  • All Research Labs
  • 3D Deep Learning
  • Applied Research
  • Autonomous Vehicles
  • Deep Imagination
  • New and Featured
  • AI Art Gallery
  • AI & Machine Learning
  • Computer Vision
  • Academic Collaborations
  • Government Collaborations
  • Graduate Fellowship
  • Internships
  • Research Openings
  • Research Scientists
  • Meet the Team
  • Publications

Kilometer-Scale Convection Allowing Model Emulation using Generative Diffusion Modeling

Publication image

Storm-scale convection-allowing models (CAMs) are an important tool for predicting the evolution of thunderstorms and mesoscale convective systems that result in damaging extreme weather. By explicitly resolving convective dynamics within the atmosphere they afford meteorologists the nuance needed to provide outlook on hazard. Deep learning models have thus far not proven skilful at km-scale atmospheric simulation, despite being competitive at coarser resolution with state-of-the-art global, medium-range weather forecasting. We present a generative diffusion model called StormCast, which emulates the high-resolution rapid refresh (HRRR) model—NOAA’s state-of-the-art 3km operational CAM. StormCast autoregressively predicts 99 state variables at km scale using a 1-hour time step, with dense vertical resolution in the atmospheric boundary layer, conditioned on 26 synoptic variables. We present evidence of successfully learnt km-scale dynamics including competitive 1-6 hour forecast skill for composite radar reflectivity alongside physically realistic convective cluster evolution, moist updrafts, and cold pool morphology. StormCast predictions maintain realistic power spectra for multiple predicted variables across multi-hour forecasts. Together, these results establish the potential for autoregressive ML to emulate CAMs – opening up new km-scale frontiers for regional ML weather prediction and future climate hazard dynamical downscaling.

Publication Date

Research area, uploaded files.

COMMENTS

  1. Research Paper Flashcards

    A research paper is the culmination and final product of an involved process of research, critical thinking, source evaluation, organization, and composition. Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like Title, Abstract, Introduction and SOP and more.

  2. Article Summarizer

    Scholarcy's AI summarization tool is designed to generate accurate, reliable article summaries. Our summarizer tool is trained to identify key terms, claims, and findings in academic papers. These insights are turned into digestible Summary Flashcards. Scroll in the box below to see the magic ⤸. The knowledge extraction and summarization ...

  3. Research Paper Flashcards

    The thesis is the ___ of a research paper. main idea. What is the fifth step to writing a research paper. organizing and outlining. what is the sixth step to writing a research paper. drafting. List three patterns of organization. spatial chronological comparison. What is the fourth step to writing a research paper.

  4. Scholarcy

    Refresh your memory. Quickly remind yourself of the key facts and findings before a lecture or meeting with your supervisor. Synthesize your insights. Export to other apps. Export your flashcards to a range of file formats that are compatible with lots of research and productivity apps.

  5. Research Paper Flashcards

    Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like Outline, Topic, Introduction and more. ... The subject of your research paper; also called a theme; what your paper is about. Introduction. The first paragraph of a writing; used to present the topic of your research paper.

  6. Scholarcy

    Try Scholarcy's Flashcard Generator today. Import your papers and chapters to generate interactive summary flashcards that highlight key information, give you the definitions of key concepts, take you straight to the cited sources, and even more: Quickly see how a study compares to earlier research. Download your references to import into ...

  7. Free Computers Flashcards about Research papers

    An artificial end to a column to balance columns or to provide space for the insertion of other objects. Study free Computers flashcards about Research papers created by Van-Neisha to improve your grades. Matching game, word search puzzle, and hangman also available.

  8. studylib.net

    Free essays, homework help, flashcards, research papers, book reports, term papers, history, science, politics

  9. Reinventing Flashcards to Increase Student Learning

    Although flashcards are relatively easy to create and commonly used by college students (Wissman et al., 2012), the effectiveness of student-created flashcards in a classroom is unclear.In an introductory psychology class, Golding et al. (2012) found that students who used flashcards for the first exam scored higher than students who did not. . However, the use of flashcards was not beneficial ...

  10. Research Methods Flashcards & Quizzes

    Research Methods. Chapter 1: Psychology Is A Way Of Thinking, Chapter 2: Sources Of Information: Evaluating, Finding, and Reading Information, Chapter 3 Three Claims, Four Validities: Interrogation Tools For Consumers Of Research ...

  11. Research Paper FlashCards Flashcards

    Research Paper FlashCards. Description. n/a. Total Cards. 19. Subject. Language - English. Level. 6th Grade. Created. 01/29/2013. Click here to study/print these flashcards. Create your own flash cards! Sign up here. Additional Language - English Flashcards . Cards Return to Set Details. Term. Purpose: Definition.

  12. Free Flashcards about research paper

    Study free flashcards about research paper created by Jacobd5 to improve your grades. Matching game, word search puzzle, and hangman also available.

  13. AI Flashcard Generator

    AI Flashcard Generator. Preparing for a test, exam, or quiz? Revisely's flashcard maker automatically transforms your pictures, notes, PDFs, PowerPoints and other documents into flashcards using the power of AI. Simply upload your materials, and our AI will create your flashcards in seconds.

  14. research paper Flashcards

    ACTS realtionship to wealth source #2 The study, published Feb. 18 on the website of the National Association for College Admission Counseling, adds new fuel to debates over the role of entrance exams in college admissions. Students in the study who did not submit SAT or ACT scores were more likely to be minorities, the first in their family to attend college, come from low-income families ...

  15. Research Paper Flashcards

    Flashcards >> English >> Research Paper; Shared Flashcard Set. Details. Title. Research Paper. Description. notecards for research paper. Total Cards. 23. Subject. English. Level. 10th Grade. Created. 03/07/2011. Click here to study/print these flashcards. Create your own flash cards! Sign up here.

  16. How to write a research paper: tips and tricks

    You get the research paper in time and offer some edits if needed. How to write a research paper: a step-by-step guide. In this section of the page, we'll give you a complete guide to write a good research paper. You will see how to craft the paper from scratch. Follow the guide from the first step to the last one or pick up only the steps ...

  17. The Research Paper Flashcards

    4.4.3 Learn with flashcards, games, and more — for free. ... Janet is writing a research paper about the use of radiation in cancer treatment. She has located a source she wants to use: an article published in a peer-reviewed journal focused on cancer research, written by David Sparks, M.D. in 1976. ...

  18. Flash Card Research Paper

    Flash Card Research Paper. 1153 Words5 Pages. THE EFFECTIVENESS OF "FLASH CARD" IN IMPROVING THE KNOWLEDGE OF MOTHERS ABOUT PREGNANCY. INTRODUCTION. Pregnancy is the growth and development of an intrauterine fetus starting from conception to the onset of true labour that marks the beginning of the intrapartum period and the duration of normal ...

  19. AI Research Tools

    Research Rabbit is currently free. Research Rabbit FAQs: Scholarcy: Scholarcy summarizes key points and claims of articles into 'summary cards' that researchers can read, share, and annotate when compiling research on a given topic. Scholarcy only uses research papers uploaded or linked by the researchers themselves. It helps you read and ...

  20. Research: How to Build Consensus Around a New Idea

    New research suggests that this rejection can be due to people's lack of shared criteria or reference points when evaluating a potential innovation's value. In a new paper, the authors find ...

  21. Study reveals the benefits and downside of fasting

    MIT postdocs Shinya Imada and Saleh Khawaled are the lead authors of the paper, which appears today in Nature. Driving regeneration. For several years, Yilmaz's lab has been investigating how fasting and low-calorie diets affect intestinal health. ... The research was funded, in part, by a Pew-Stewart Trust Scholar award, the Marble Center ...

  22. Research Finds US, China Drive Global Divide in AI Chip Power

    The paper finds that the U.S. and China have by far the most public GPU clusters in the world. China leads the U.S. on the number of GPU-enabled regions overall, however the most advanced GPUs are ...

  23. The physics behind the most annoying thing that could ever happen to

    Scientists have figured out what type of paper is the most prone to cut skin. Kaare Jensen, associate professor of physics at the Technical University of Denmark, explains.

  24. Elon professor wins award for AI and consumer behavior research

    Assistant Professor Smaraki Mohanty at the Martha and Spencer Love School of Business received two awards, Best Overall Paper and Best in Track Paper, at the AMA Summer Academic Conference in August 2024 for her co-authored research. The paper titled, "Political Ideology and Acceptance of AI-Based Recommendations," explores how political beliefs could influence the acceptance of AI-driven ...

  25. Research paper Flashcards

    Start studying Research paper. Learn vocabulary, terms, and more with flashcards, games, and other study tools.

  26. Research shows the ages our metabolism undergoes massive rapid changes

    Research shows our bodies go through rapid changes in our 40s and our 60s. ... "The beauty of this type of paper is the level of detail we can see in molecular changes," said Coresh, a ...

  27. 27 August 2024 15:30-17:00 Economics Revision (Paper 2) Grade 12

    About Press Copyright Contact us Creators Advertise Developers Terms Privacy Policy & Safety How YouTube works Test new features NFL Sunday Ticket Press Copyright ...

  28. Kilometer-Scale Convection Allowing Model Emulation using Generative

    Storm-scale convection-allowing models (CAMs) are an important tool for predicting the evolution of thunderstorms and mesoscale convective systems that result in damaging extreme weather. By explicitly resolving convective dynamics within the atmosphere they afford meteorologists the nuance needed to provide outlook on hazard. Deep learning models have thus far not proven skilful at km-scale ...

  29. Research Paper Reading Quiz Flashcards

    Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like What is the basic purpose of a research paper?, What should a good research paper do? And, what should you do before you begin to look for information?, What information should a source card include? and more.

  30. Nestle to Keep Health Science Unit After CEO's Exit, Chairman Tells Paper

    ZURICH (Reuters) -Nestle intends to keep its Health Science unit, Chairman Paul Bulcke said in a newspaper interview published on Sunday, after the food giant announced it was replacing its CEO ...