Purdue Online Writing Lab Purdue OWL® College of Liberal Arts

APA Sample Paper: Experimental Psychology

OWL logo

Welcome to the Purdue OWL

This page is brought to you by the OWL at Purdue University. When printing this page, you must include the entire legal notice.

Copyright ©1995-2018 by The Writing Lab & The OWL at Purdue and Purdue University. All rights reserved. This material may not be published, reproduced, broadcast, rewritten, or redistributed without permission. Use of this site constitutes acceptance of our terms and conditions of fair use.

American Psychological Association Logo

Sample articles

Journal of experimental psychology: general ®.

  • The Role of Political Devotion in Sharing Partisan Misinformation and Resistance to Fact-Checking (PDF, 3.3MB) June 2023 by Clara Pretus, Camila Servin-Barthet, Elizabeth A. Harris, William J. Brady, Oscar Vilarroya, and Jay J. Van Bavel
  • The Secret to Happiness: Feeling Good or Feeling Right? (PDF, 171KB) October 2017 by Maya Tamir, Shalom H. Schwartz, Shige Oishi, and Min Y. Kim
  • Overdistribution Illusions: Categorical Judgments Produce Them, Confidence Ratings Reduce Them (PDF, 182KB) January 2017 by C. J. Brainerd, K. Nakamura, V. F. Reyna, and R. E. Holliday
  • Motivated Recall in the Service of the Economic System: The Case of Anthropogenic Climate Change (PDF, 197KB) June 2016 by Erin P. Hennes, Benjamin C. Ruisch, Irina Feygina, Christopher A. Monteiro, and John T. Jost
  • Handwriting Generates Variable Visual Output to Facilitate Symbol Learning (PDF, 234KB) March 2016 by Julia X. Li and Karin H. James
  • Perceptual Dehumanization of Faces Is Activated by Norm Violations and Facilitates Norm Enforcement (PDF, 262KB) February 2016 by Katrina M. Fincher and Philip E. Tetlock
  • Moving While Black: Intergroup Attitudes Influence Judgments of Speed (PDF, 71KB) February 2016 by Andreana C. Kenrick, Stacey Sinclair, Jennifer Richeson, Sara C. Verosky, and Janetta Lun
  • Searching for Explanations: How the Internet Inflates Estimates of Internal Knowledge (PDF, 138KB) June 2015 by Matthew Fisher, Mariel K. Goddu, and Frank C. Keil
  • Finding a Needle in a Haystack: Toward a Psychologically Informed Method for Aviation Security Screening (PDF, 129KB) February 2015 by Thomas C. Ormerod and Coral J. Dando
  • The Myth of Harmless Wrongs in Moral Cognition: Automatic Dyadic Completion From Sin to Suffering (PDF, 197KB) August 2014 by Kurt Gray, Chelsea Schein, and Adrian F. Ward
  • Get Excited: Reappraising Pre-Performance Anxiety as Excitement (PDF, 217KB) June 2014 by Alison Wood Brooks
  • Set-Fit Effects in Choice (PDF, 93KB) April 2014 by Ellen R. K. Evers, Yoel Inbar, and Marcel Zeelenberg
  • I Want to Help You, But I Am Not Sure Why: Gaze-Cuing Induces Altruistic Giving (PDF, 190KB) April 2014 by Robert D. Rogers, Andrew P. Bayliss, Anna Szepietowska, Laura Dale, Lydia Reeder, Gloria Pizzamiglio, Karolina Czarna, Judi Wakeley, Phillip J. Cowen, Judi Wakeley and Phillip J. Cowen
  • Breaking the Cycle of Mistrust: Wise Interventions to Provide Critical Feedback Across the Racial Divide (PDF, 366KB) April 2014 by David Scott Yeager, Valerie Purdie-Vaughns, Julio Garcia, Nancy Apfel, Patti Brzustoski, Allison Master, William T. Hessert, Matthew E. Williams, and Geoffrey L. Cohen
  • Consolidation Power of Extrinsic Rewards: Reward Cues Enhance Long-Term Memory for Irrelevant Past Events (PDF, 87KB) February 2014 by Kou Murayama and Shinji Kitagami
  • The Invisible Man: Interpersonal Goals Moderate Inattentional Blindness to African Americans (PDF, 83KB) February 2014 by Jazmin L. Brown-Iannuzzi, Kelly M. Hoffman, B. Keith Payne, and Sophie Trawalter
  • Learning to Contend With Accents in Infancy: Benefits of Brief Speaker Exposure (PDF, 145KB) February 2014 by Marieke van Heugten and Elizabeth K. Johnson
  • On Feeding Those Hungry for Praise: Person Praise Backfires in Children With Low Self-Esteem (PDF, 102KB) February 2014 by Eddie Brummelman, Sander Thomaes, Geertjan Overbeek, Bram Orobio de Castro, Marcel A. van den Hout, and Brad J. Bushman
  • Paying It Forward: Generalized Reciprocity and the Limits of Generosity (PDF, 256KB) February 2014 by Kurt Gray, Adrian F. Ward, and Michael I. Norton
  • Shape Beyond Recognition: Form-Derived Directionality and Its Effects on Visual Attention and Motion Perception (PDF, 336KB) February 2014 by Heida M. Sigurdardottir, Suzanne M. Michalak, and David L. Sheinberg
  • Consonance and Pitch (PDF, 380KB) November 2013 by Neil McLachlan, David Marco, Maria Light, and Sarah Wilson
  • Forgetting Our Personal Past: Socially Shared Retrieval-Induced Forgetting of Autobiographical Memories (PDF, 126KB) November 2013 by Charles B. Stone, Amanda J. Barnier, John Sutton, and William Hirst
  • Preventing Motor Skill Failure Through Hemisphere-Specific Priming: Cases From Choking Under Pressure (PDF, 129KB) August 2013 by Jürgen Beckmann, Peter Gröpel, and Felix Ehrlenspiel
  • How Decisions Emerge: Action Dynamics in Intertemporal Decision Making (PDF, 206KB) February 2013 by Maja Dshemuchadse, Stefan Scherbaum, and Thomas Goschke
  • Improving Working Memory Efficiency by Reframing Metacognitive Interpretation of Task Difficulty (PDF, 110KB) November 2012 by Frédérique Autin and Jean-Claude Croizet
  • Divine Intuition: Cognitive Style Influences Belief in God (PDF, 102KB) August 2012 by Amitai Shenhav, David G. Rand, and Joshua D. Greene
  • Internal Representations Reveal Cultural Diversity in Expectations of Facial Expressions of Emotion (PDF, 320KB) February 2012 by Rachael E. Jack, Roberto Caldara, and Philippe G. Schyns
  • The Pain Was Greater If It Will Happen Again: The Effect of Anticipated Continuation on Retrospective Discomfort (PDF, 148KB) February 2011 by Jeff Galak and Tom Meyvis
  • The Nature of Gestures' Beneficial Role in Spatial Problem Solving (PDF, 181KB) February 2011 by Mingyuan Chu and Sotaro Kita

More about this journal

  • Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  • Pricing and subscription info

Contact Journals

  • - Google Chrome

Intended for healthcare professionals

  • My email alerts
  • BMA member login
  • Username * Password * Forgot your log in details? Need to activate BMA Member Log In Log in via OpenAthens Log in via your institution

Home

Search form

  • Advanced search
  • Search responses
  • Search blogs
  • Beauty sleep:...

Beauty sleep: experimental study on the perceived health and attractiveness of sleep deprived people

  • Related content
  • Peer review
  • John Axelsson , researcher 1 2 ,
  • Tina Sundelin , research assistant and MSc student 2 ,
  • Michael Ingre , statistician and PhD student 3 ,
  • Eus J W Van Someren , researcher 4 ,
  • Andreas Olsson , researcher 2 ,
  • Mats Lekander , researcher 1 3
  • 1 Osher Center for Integrative Medicine, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, 17177 Stockholm, Sweden
  • 2 Division for Psychology, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet
  • 3 Stress Research Institute, Stockholm University, Stockholm
  • 4 Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, an Institute of the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, and VU Medical Center, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • Correspondence to: J Axelsson john.axelsson{at}ki.se
  • Accepted 22 October 2010

Objective To investigate whether sleep deprived people are perceived as less healthy, less attractive, and more tired than after a normal night’s sleep.

Design Experimental study.

Setting Sleep laboratory in Stockholm, Sweden.

Participants 23 healthy, sleep deprived adults (age 18-31) who were photographed and 65 untrained observers (age 18-61) who rated the photographs.

Intervention Participants were photographed after a normal night’s sleep (eight hours) and after sleep deprivation (31 hours of wakefulness after a night of reduced sleep). The photographs were presented in a randomised order and rated by untrained observers.

Main outcome measure Difference in observer ratings of perceived health, attractiveness, and tiredness between sleep deprived and well rested participants using a visual analogue scale (100 mm).

Results Sleep deprived people were rated as less healthy (visual analogue scale scores, mean 63 (SE 2) v 68 (SE 2), P<0.001), more tired (53 (SE 3) v 44 (SE 3), P<0.001), and less attractive (38 (SE 2) v 40 (SE 2), P<0.001) than after a normal night’s sleep. The decrease in rated health was associated with ratings of increased tiredness and decreased attractiveness.

Conclusion Our findings show that sleep deprived people appear less healthy, less attractive, and more tired compared with when they are well rested. This suggests that humans are sensitive to sleep related facial cues, with potential implications for social and clinical judgments and behaviour. Studies are warranted for understanding how these effects may affect clinical decision making and can add knowledge with direct implications in a medical context.

Introduction

The recognition [of the case] depends in great measure on the accurate and rapid appreciation of small points in which the diseased differs from the healthy state Joseph Bell (1837-1911)

Good clinical judgment is an important skill in medical practice. This is well illustrated in the quote by Joseph Bell, 1 who demonstrated impressive observational and deductive skills. Bell was one of Sir Arthur Conan Doyle’s teachers and served as a model for the fictitious detective Sherlock Holmes. 2 Generally, human judgment involves complex processes, whereby ingrained, often less consciously deliberated responses from perceptual cues are mixed with semantic calculations to affect decision making. 3 Thus all social interactions, including diagnosis in clinical practice, are influenced by reflexive as well as reflective processes in human cognition and communication.

Sleep is an essential homeostatic process with well established effects on an individual’s physiological, cognitive, and behavioural functionality 4 5 6 7 and long term health, 8 but with only anecdotal support of a role in social perception, such as that underlying judgments of attractiveness and health. As illustrated by the common expression “beauty sleep,” an individual’s sleep history may play an integral part in the perception and judgments of his or her attractiveness and health. To date, the concept of beauty sleep has lacked scientific support, but the biological importance of sleep may have favoured a sensitivity to perceive sleep related cues in others. It seems warranted to explore such sensitivity, as sleep disorders and disturbed sleep are increasingly common in today’s 24 hour society and often coexist with some of the most common health problems, such as hypertension 9 10 and inflammatory conditions. 11

To describe the relation between sleep deprivation and perceived health and attractiveness we asked untrained observers to rate the faces of people who had been photographed after a normal night’s sleep and after a night of sleep deprivation. We chose facial photographs as the human face is the primary source of information in social communication. 12 A perceiver’s response to facial cues, signalling the bearer’s emotional state, intentions, and potential mate value, serves to guide actions in social contexts and may ultimately promote survival. 13 14 15 We hypothesised that untrained observers would perceive sleep deprived people as more tired, less healthy, and less attractive compared with after a normal night’s sleep.

Using an experimental design we photographed the faces of 23 adults (mean age 23, range 18-31 years, 11 women) between 14.00 and 15.00 under two conditions in a balanced design: after a normal night’s sleep (at least eight hours of sleep between 23.00-07.00 and seven hours of wakefulness) and after sleep deprivation (sleep 02.00-07.00 and 31 hours of wakefulness). We advertised for participants at four universities in the Stockholm area. Twenty of 44 potentially eligible people were excluded. Reasons for exclusion were reported sleep disturbances, abnormal sleep requirements (for example, sleep need out of the 7-9 hour range), health problems, or availability on study days (the main reason). We also excluded smokers and those who had consumed alcohol within two days of the protocol. One woman failed to participate in both conditions. Overall, we enrolled 12 women and 12 men.

The participants slept in their own homes. Sleep times were confirmed with sleep diaries and text messages. The sleep diaries (Karolinska sleep diary) included information on sleep latency, quality, duration, and sleepiness. Participants sent a text message to the research assistant by mobile phone (SMS) at bedtime and when they got up on the night before sleep deprivation. They had been instructed not to nap. During the normal sleep condition the participants’ mean duration of sleep, estimated from sleep diaries, was 8.45 (SE 0.20) hours. The sleep deprivation condition started with a restriction of sleep to five hours in bed; the participants sent text messages (SMS) when they went to sleep and when they woke up. The mean duration of sleep during this night, estimated from sleep diaries and text messages, was 5.06 (SE 0.04) hours. For the following night of total sleep deprivation, the participants were monitored in the sleep laboratory at all times. Thus, for the sleep deprivation condition, participants came to the laboratory at 22.00 (after 15 hours of wakefulness) to be monitored, and stayed awake for a further 16 hours. We therefore did not observe the participants during the first 15 hours of wakefulness, when they had had a slightly restricted sleep, but had good control over the last 16 hours of wakefulness when sleepiness increased in magnitude. For the sleep condition, participants came to the laboratory at 12.00 (after five hours of wakefulness). They were kept indoors two hours before being photographed to avoid the effects of exposure to sunlight and the weather. We had a series of five or six photographs (resolution 3872×2592 pixels) taken in a well lit room, with a constant white balance (×900l; colour temperature 4200 K, Nikon D80; Nikon, Tokyo). The white balance was differently set during the two days of the study and affected seven photographs (four taken during sleep deprivation and three during a normal night’s sleep). Removing these participants from the analyses did not affect the results. The distance from camera to head was fixed, as was the focal length, within 14 mm (between 44 and 58 mm). To ensure a fixed surface area of each face on the photograph, the focal length was adapted to the head size of each participant.

For the photo shoot, participants wore no makeup, had their hair loose (combed backwards if long), underwent similar cleaning or shaving procedures for both conditions, and were instructed to “sit with a straight back and look straight into the camera with a neutral, relaxed facial expression.” Although the photographer was not blinded to the sleep conditions, she followed a highly standardised procedure during each photo shoot, including minimal interaction with the participants. A blinded rater chose the most typical photograph from each series of photographs. This process resulted in 46 photographs; two (one from each sleep condition) of each of the 23 participants. This part of the study took place between June and September 2007.

In October 2007 the photographs were presented at a fixed interval of six seconds in a randomised order to 65 observers (mainly students at the Karolinska Institute, mean age 30 (range 18-61) years, 40 women), who were unaware of the conditions of the study. They rated the faces for attractiveness (very unattractive to very attractive), health (very sick to very healthy), and tiredness (not at all tired to very tired) on a 100 mm visual analogue scale. After every 23 photographs a brief intermission was allowed, including a working memory task lasting 23 seconds to prevent the faces being memorised. To ensure that the observers were not primed to tiredness when rating health and attractiveness they rated the photographs for attractiveness and health in the first two sessions and tiredness in the last. To avoid the influence of possible order effects we presented the photographs in a balanced order between conditions for each session.

Statistical analyses

Data were analysed using multilevel mixed effects linear regression, with two crossed independent random effects accounting for random variation between observers and participants using the xtmixed procedure in Stata 9.2. We present the effect of condition as a percentage of change from the baseline condition as the reference using the absolute value in millimetres (rated on the visual analogue scale). No data were missing in the analyses.

Sixty five observers rated each of the 46 photographs for attractiveness, health, and tiredness: 138 ratings by each observer and 2990 ratings for each of the three factors rated. When sleep deprived, people were rated as less healthy (visual analogue scale scores, mean 63 (SE 2) v 68 (SE 2)), more tired (53 (SE 3) v 44 (SE 3)), and less attractive (38 (SE 2) v 40 (SE 2); P<0.001 for all) than after a normal night’s sleep (table 1 ⇓ ). Compared with the normal sleep condition, perceptions of health and attractiveness in the sleep deprived condition decreased on average by 6% and 4% and tiredness increased by 19%.

 Multilevel mixed effects regression on effect of how sleep deprived people are perceived with respect to attractiveness, health, and tiredness

  • View inline

A 10 mm increase in tiredness was associated with a −3.0 mm change in health, a 10 mm increase in health increased attractiveness by 2.4 mm, and a 10 mm increase in tiredness reduced attractiveness by 1.2 mm (table 2 ⇓ ). These findings were also presented as correlation, suggesting that faces with perceived attractiveness are positively associated with perceived health (r=0.42, fig 1 ⇓ ) and negatively with perceived tiredness (r=−0.28, fig 1). In addition, the average decrease (for each face) in attractiveness as a result of deprived sleep was associated with changes in tiredness (−0.53, n=23, P=0.03) and in health (0.50, n=23, P=0.01). Moreover, a strong negative association was found between the respective perceptions of tiredness and health (r=−0.54, fig 1). Figure 2 ⇓ shows an example of observer rated faces.

 Associations between health, tiredness, and attractiveness

Fig 1  Relations between health, tiredness, and attractiveness of 46 photographs (two each of 23 participants) rated by 65 observers on 100 mm visual analogue scales, with variation between observers removed using empirical Bayes’ estimates

  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint

Fig 2  Participant after a normal night’s sleep (left) and after sleep deprivation (right). Faces were presented in a counterbalanced order

To evaluate the mediation effects of sleep loss on attractiveness and health, tiredness was added to the models presented in table 1 following recommendations. 16 The effect of sleep loss was significantly mediated by tiredness on both health (P<0.001) and attractiveness (P<0.001). When tiredness was added to the model (table 1) with an estimated coefficient of −2.9 (SE 0.1; P<0.001) the independent effect of sleep loss on health decreased from −4.2 to −1.8 (SE 0.5; P<0.001). The effect of sleep loss on attractiveness decreased from −1.6 (table 1) to −0.62 (SE 0.4; P=0.133), with tiredness estimated at −1.1 (SE 0.1; P<0.001). The same approach applied to the model of attractiveness and health (table 2), with a decrease in the association from 2.4 to 2.1 (SE 0.1; P<0.001) with tiredness estimated at −0.56 (SE 0.1; P<0.001).

Sleep deprived people are perceived as less attractive, less healthy, and more tired compared with when they are well rested. Apparent tiredness was strongly related to looking less healthy and less attractive, which was also supported by the mediating analyses, indicating that a large part of the found effects and relations on appearing healthy and attractive were mediated by looking tired. The fact that untrained observers detected the effects of sleep loss in others not only provides evidence for a perceptual ability not previously subjected to experimental control, but also supports the notion that sleep history gives rise to socially relevant signals that provide information about the bearer. The adaptiveness of an ability to detect sleep related facial cues resonates well with other research, showing that small deviations from the average sleep duration in the long term are associated with an increased risk of health problems and with a decreased longevity. 8 17 Indeed, even a few hours of sleep deprivation inflict an array of physiological changes, including neural, endocrinological, immunological, and cellular functioning, that if sustained are relevant for long term health. 7 18 19 20 Here, we show that such physiological changes are paralleled by detectable facial changes.

These results are related to photographs taken in an artificial setting and presented to the observers for only six seconds. It is likely that the effects reported here would be larger in real life person to person situations, when overt behaviour and interactions add further information. Blink interval and blink duration are known to be indicators of sleepiness, 21 and trained observers are able to evaluate reliably the drowsiness of drivers by watching their videotaped faces. 22 In addition, a few of the people were perceived as healthier, less tired, and more attractive during the sleep deprived condition. It remains to be evaluated in follow-up research whether this is due to random error noise in judgments, or associated with specific characteristics of observers or the sleep deprived people they judge. Nevertheless, we believe that the present findings can be generalised to a wide variety of settings, but further studies will have to investigate the impact on clinical studies and other social situations.

Importantly, our findings suggest a prominent role of sleep history in several domains of interpersonal perception and judgment, in which sleep history has previously not been considered of importance, such as in clinical judgment. In addition, because attractiveness motivates sexual behaviour, collaboration, and superior treatment, 13 sleep loss may have consequences in other social contexts. For example, it has been proposed that facial cues perceived as attractive are signals of good health and that this recognition has been selected evolutionarily to guide choice of mate and successful transmission of genes. 13 The fact that good sleep supports a healthy look and poor sleep the reverse may be of particular relevance in the medical setting, where health estimates are an essential part. It is possible that people with sleep disturbances, clinical or otherwise, would be judged as more unhealthy, whereas those who have had an unusually good night’s sleep may be perceived as rather healthy. Compared with the sleep deprivation used in the present investigation, further studies are needed to investigate the effects of less drastic acute reductions of sleep as well as long term clinical effects.

Conclusions

People are capable of detecting sleep loss related facial cues, and these cues modify judgments of another’s health and attractiveness. These conclusions agree well with existing models describing a link between sleep and good health, 18 23 as well as a link between attractiveness and health. 13 Future studies should focus on the relevance of these facial cues in clinical settings. These could investigate whether clinicians are better than the average population at detecting sleep or health related facial cues, and whether patients with a clinical diagnosis exhibit more tiredness and are less healthy looking than healthy people. Perhaps the more successful doctors are those who pick up on these details and act accordingly.

Taken together, our results provide important insights into judgments about health and attractiveness that are reminiscent of the anecdotal wisdom harboured in Bell’s words, and in the colloquial notion of “beauty sleep.”

What is already known on this topic

Short or disturbed sleep and fatigue constitute major risk factors for health and safety

Complaints of short or disturbed sleep are common among patients seeking healthcare

The human face is the main source of information for social signalling

What this study adds

The facial cues of sleep deprived people are sufficient for others to judge them as more tired, less healthy, and less attractive, lending the first scientific support to the concept of “beauty sleep”

By affecting doctors’ general perception of health, the sleep history of a patient may affect clinical decisions and diagnostic precision

Cite this as: BMJ 2010;341:c6614

We thank B Karshikoff for support with data acquisition and M Ingvar for comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript, both without compensation and working at the Department for Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden.

Contributors: JA designed the data collection, supervised and monitored data collection, wrote the statistical analysis plan, carried out the statistical analyses, obtained funding, drafted and revised the manuscript, and is guarantor. TS designed and carried out the data collection, cleaned the data, drafted, revised the manuscript, and had final approval of the manuscript. JA and TS contributed equally to the work. MI wrote the statistical analysis plan, carried out the statistical analyses, drafted the manuscript, and critically revised the manuscript. EJWVS provided statistical advice, advised on data handling, and critically revised the manuscript. AO provided advice on the methods and critically revised the manuscript. ML provided administrative support, drafted the manuscript, and critically revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding: This study was funded by the Swedish Society for Medical Research, Rut and Arvid Wolff’s Memory Fund, and the Osher Center for Integrative Medicine.

Competing interests: All authors have completed the Unified Competing Interest form at www.icmje.org/coi_disclosure.pdf (available on request from the corresponding author) and declare: no support from any company for the submitted work; no financial relationships with any companies that might have an interest in the submitted work in the previous 3 years; no other relationships or activities that could appear to have influenced the submitted work.

Ethical approval: This study was approved by the Karolinska Institutet’s ethical committee. Participants were compensated for their participation.

Participant consent: Participant’s consent obtained.

Data sharing: Statistical code and dataset of ratings are available from the corresponding author at john.axelsson{at}ki.se .

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-commercial License, which permits use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non commercial and is otherwise in compliance with the license. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/2.0/ and http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/2.0/legalcode .

  • ↵ Deten A, Volz HC, Clamors S, Leiblein S, Briest W, Marx G, et al. Hematopoietic stem cells do not repair the infarcted mouse heart. Cardiovasc Res 2005 ; 65 : 52 -63. OpenUrl Abstract / FREE Full Text
  • ↵ Doyle AC. The case-book of Sherlock Holmes: selected stories. Wordsworth, 1993.
  • ↵ Lieberman MD, Gaunt R, Gilbert DT, Trope Y. Reflection and reflexion: a social cognitive neuroscience approach to attributional inference. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 2002 ; 34 : 199 -249. OpenUrl CrossRef
  • ↵ Drummond SPA, Brown GG, Gillin JC, Stricker JL, Wong EC, Buxton RB. Altered brain response to verbal learning following sleep deprivation. Nature 2000 ; 403 : 655 -7. OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed
  • ↵ Harrison Y, Horne JA. The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: a review. J Exp Psychol Appl 2000 ; 6 : 236 -49. OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Web of Science
  • ↵ Huber R, Ghilardi MF, Massimini M, Tononi G. Local sleep and learning. Nature 2004 ; 430 : 78 -81. OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Web of Science
  • ↵ Spiegel K, Leproult R, Van Cauter E. Impact of sleep debt on metabolic and endocrine function. Lancet 1999 ; 354 : 1435 -9. OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Web of Science
  • ↵ Kripke DF, Garfinkel L, Wingard DL, Klauber MR, Marler MR. Mortality associated with sleep duration and insomnia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002 ; 59 : 131 -6. OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Web of Science
  • ↵ Olson LG, Ambrogetti A. Waking up to sleep disorders. Br J Hosp Med (Lond) 2006 ; 67 : 118 , 20. OpenUrl PubMed
  • ↵ Rajaratnam SM, Arendt J. Health in a 24-h society. Lancet 2001 ; 358 : 999 -1005. OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Web of Science
  • ↵ Ranjbaran Z, Keefer L, Stepanski E, Farhadi A, Keshavarzian A. The relevance of sleep abnormalities to chronic inflammatory conditions. Inflamm Res 2007 ; 56 : 51 -7. OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Web of Science
  • ↵ Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI. The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn Sci 2000 ; 4 : 223 -33. OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Web of Science
  • ↵ Rhodes G. The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annu Rev Psychol 2006 ; 57 : 199 -226. OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Web of Science
  • ↵ Todorov A, Mandisodza AN, Goren A, Hall CC. Inferences of competence from faces predict election outcomes. Science 2005 ; 308 : 1623 -6. OpenUrl Abstract / FREE Full Text
  • ↵ Willis J, Todorov A. First impressions: making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychol Sci 2006 ; 17 : 592 -8. OpenUrl Abstract / FREE Full Text
  • ↵ Krull JL, MacKinnon DP. Multilevel modeling of individual and group level mediated effects. Multivariate Behav Res 2001 ; 36 : 249 -77. OpenUrl CrossRef Web of Science
  • ↵ Ayas NT, White DP, Manson JE, Stampfer MJ, Speizer FE, Malhotra A, et al. A prospective study of sleep duration and coronary heart disease in women. Arch Intern Med 2003 ; 163 : 205 -9. OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Web of Science
  • ↵ Bryant PA, Trinder J, Curtis N. Sick and tired: does sleep have a vital role in the immune system. Nat Rev Immunol 2004 ; 4 : 457 -67. OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Web of Science
  • ↵ Cirelli C. Cellular consequences of sleep deprivation in the brain. Sleep Med Rev 2006 ; 10 : 307 -21. OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Web of Science
  • ↵ Irwin MR, Wang M, Campomayor CO, Collado-Hidalgo A, Cole S. Sleep deprivation and activation of morning levels of cellular and genomic markers of inflammation. Arch Intern Med 2006 ; 166 : 1756 -62. OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Web of Science
  • ↵ Schleicher R, Galley N, Briest S, Galley L. Blinks and saccades as indicators of fatigue in sleepiness warnings: looking tired? Ergonomics 2008 ; 51 : 982 -1010. OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Web of Science
  • ↵ Wierwille WW, Ellsworth LA. Evaluation of driver drowsiness by trained raters. Accid Anal Prev 1994 ; 26 : 571 -81. OpenUrl CrossRef PubMed Web of Science
  • ↵ Horne J. Why we sleep—the functions of sleep in humans and other mammals. Oxford University Press, 1988.

experimental research paper sample pdf

Exploring Experimental Research: Methodologies, Designs, and Applications Across Disciplines

9 Pages Posted: 23 Apr 2024

Sereyrath Em

National University of Cheasim Kamchaymear, Kampong Cham, Cambodia; Suranaree University of Technology Nakhon Ratchasima, Thailand

Date Written: 2024

Experimental research serves as a fundamental scientific method aimed at unraveling cause-and-effect relationships between variables across various disciplines. This paper delineates the key features of experimental research, including the manipulation of variables, controlled conditions, random assignment, and meticulous measurement techniques to facilitate causal inferences. It elucidates different experimental designs such as randomized controlled trials, true experimental designs, quasi-experimental designs, and single-case designs, each tailored to specific research contexts. Moreover, the paper expounds on the procedural steps in conducting experimental research, emphasizing the importance of methodological rigor from study design to result interpretation. Additionally, it delineates the potential threats to internal and external validity, highlighting the significance of mitigating confounding factors for robust experimental outcomes. Furthermore, the paper discusses the timing of pre-tests and post-tests, the intricacies of experiment design, and emerging trends such as internet-based experiments and ex post facto research. Through a comprehensive examination of experimental research methodologies, designs, and applications, this paper aims to provide researchers with a nuanced understanding of experimental inquiry across diverse academic domains.

Keywords: Experimental Research, Causal Inference, Research Designs, Internal Validity, External Validity

Suggested Citation: Suggested Citation

Sereyrath Em (Contact Author)

National university of cheasim kamchaymear, kampong cham, cambodia ( email ).

Kampong Cham, Cambodia Phnom Penh, 855 Cambodia

Suranaree University of Technology Nakhon Ratchasima, Thailand ( email )

Nakhon, Ratchasima, 3000, Thailand Thailand

Do you have a job opening that you would like to promote on SSRN?

Paper statistics, related ejournals, randomized social experiments ejournal.

Subscribe to this fee journal for more curated articles on this topic

Educational Impact & Evaluation Research eJournal

Subscribe to this free journal for more curated articles on this topic

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH METHODS

Profile image of Steven Ross

Related Papers

Journal of Computing in Higher Education

Gary Morrison

experimental research paper sample pdf

Cut Eka Para Samya

In Part 4, we begin a more detailed discussion of some of the methodologies that educational researchers use. We concentrate here on quantitative research, with a separate chapter devoted to group-comparison experimental research, single-subject experimental research, correlational research, causal-comparative research, and survey research. In each chapter, we not only discuss the method in some detail, but we also provide examples of published studies in which the researchers used one of these methods. We conclude each chapter with an analysis of a particular study's strengths and weaknesses.

Educational Technology Research and Development

Open Access Publishing Group

The experimental method is more exact compared to other quantitative methods, although it can have drawbacks, related to the positivist epistemological position. Determining exactly the educational effects of existing, innovative and new pedagogical concepts, programmes, systems, models, methods and instruments commands the use of experiments in pedagogical research. If completed pedagogical research projects are analysed, the conclusion is experiments are used much less frequently than other methods. This study determines the prevalence of parallel-group designs as compared to how frequently other experimental designs are used. A representative sample of scientific and professional papers was analysed and it was ascertained that the conducted experiments partly satisfy relevant theoretical and methodological criteria. It is evident that result reliability when using the experimental method is still relatively low, which may have negative effects on the development of pedagogical sciences and related scientific disciplines, as well as on scientifically grounded innovation of the teaching and learning process and enhancement of the educational process. Hence, it is crucial to use multi-method research approaches (employing the experimental method as appropriate, depending on the research problem) in preparing (and approving) doctoral dissertations, writing reviews and publishing research papers.

Mind, Brain, and Education

Melina Uncapher

Thomas C Reeves

Several years ago, Professor Dave Merrill from Utah State University drew a metaphorical line in the sand that called for anyone committed to serious instructional technology research to join him and his associates in pursuing an empirical research agenda based upon the fact that instruction is a science (Merrill, Drake, Lacey, & Pratt, 1996). He also contended that instructional design is a technology derived from the science of instruction based upon principles that could be verified by empirical data.

Electronic Journal of e-Learning

Jodie Jenkinson

Educational Researcher

Richard Lehrer

Robert Tennyson

Loading Preview

Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.

RELATED PAPERS

Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, &amp Computers

Stephen Hirtle

EIKI Journal of Effective Teaching Methods

Nataliya Bhinder

15th Annual Computing and Philosophy conference, …

Saul Fisher

British Educational Research Journal

Emma Marsden

… of Continuing Engineering Education and Life …

Claus Witfelt

Mary Granger

British Journal of Educational Psychology

John Nesbit

… environments: IFIP TC3 …

Panagiotis Metaxas

AIP Conference …

Paula Engelhardt

Robert Ronau

The Journal of Technology Studies

Karen Kortecamp

Marja Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen

Ayrton Zarb

Teachers College Record

tamar almog

Steve Joordens

Contemporary Issues in Technology and …

Kara Dawson

Computers in the Schools

walter heinecke

Leonard Amiel

DR. SULAIMAN HASHIM

Mindy Kalchman

phpderslerim.com

Mustafa Serdar Köksal

Journal of Research on Technology in Education

Damian Bebell

Andy Gibbons , Jason McDonald

British Journal of Educational Technology

J. Michael Spector

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

Experimental Research Design

  • First Online: 10 November 2021

Cite this chapter

experimental research paper sample pdf

  • Stefan Hunziker 3 &
  • Michael Blankenagel 3  

3471 Accesses

1 Citations

This chapter addresses the peculiarities, characteristics, and major fallacies of experimental research designs. Experiments have a long and important history in the social, natural, and medicinal sciences. Unfortunately, in business and management this looks differently. This is astounding, as experiments are suitable for analyzing cause-and-effect relationships. A true experiment is a brilliant method for finding out if one element really causes other elements. Also, researchers find relevant information on how to write an experimental research design paper and learn about typical methodologies used for this research design. The chapter closes with referring to overlapping and adjacent research designs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Aronson, E., Ellsworth, P., Carlsmith, J., & Gonzales, M. (1990). Methods of research in social psychology (2nd ed.). McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar  

Bargh, J., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology., 71 (2), 230–244.

Article   Google Scholar  

Biais, B., & Weber, M. (2009). Hindsight bias, risk perception, and investment performance. Management Science, 55 (6), 1018–1029.

Bortz, J. & Döring, N. (2006). Forschungsmethoden und evaluation . Springer.

Bowlin, K. O., Hales, J., & Kachelmeier, S. J. (2009). Experimental evidence of how prior experience as an auditor influences managers’ strategic reporting decisions. Rev Account Stud, 14 (1), 63–87.

Burmeister, K., & Schade, C. (2007). Are entrepreneurs’ decisions more biased? An experimental investigation of the susceptibility to status quo bias. Journal of Business Venturing, 22 (3), 340–362.

Christensen, L. B. (2007). Experimental methodology . Pearson/Allyn and Bacon.

de Vaus, D. A. (2001). Research design in social research . Reprinted. Los Angeles: SAGE Publications, Inc.

Doyle, A. C. (1890). The sign of the four. Lippincott’s Monthly Magazine . Ward, Lock & Co.

Franke, N., Schreier, M., & Kaiser, U. (2010). The “I designed it myself” effect in mass customization. Management Science, 56 (1), 125–140.

Friese, M., Wilhelm, H., & Michaela, W. (2009). The impulsive consumer: Predicting consumer behavior with implicit reaction time measurements. Social Psychology of Consumer Behavior (pp. 335–364). Psychology Press.

Fromkin, H. L., & Streufert, S. (1976). Laboratory experimentation. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 415–465). Rand McNally College.

Harbring, C., & Irlenbusch, B. (2011). Sabotage in tournaments: evidence from a laboratory experiment. Management Science, 57 (4), 611–627.

Hennig-Thurau, T., Groth, M., Paul, M., & Gremler, D. D. (2006). Are all smiles created equal? How emotional contagion and emotional labor affect service relationships. Journal of Marketing, 70 (3), 58–73.

Homburg, C., Koschate, N., & Hoyer, W. D. (2005). Do Satisfied customers really pay more? A study of the relationship between customer satisfaction and willingness to pay. Journal of Marketing, 69 (2), 84–96.

Johnson, B. (2001). Toward a new classification of nonexperimental quantitative research. Educational Researcher, 30 (2), 3–13.

Koschate-Fischer, N., & Schandelmeier, S. (2014). A Guideline for Designing Experimental Studies in Marketing research and a Critical Discussion of Selected Problem Areas. Journal of Business Economics, 84 (6), 793–826.

Krishnaswamy K. N., Sivakumar A. I. & Mathirajan M. (2009). Management research methodology: Integration of principles, methods and techniques . Dorling Kindersley.

Lödding, H., & Lohmann, S. (2012). INCAP – applying short-term flexibility to control inventories. International Journal of Production Research, 50 (3), 909–919.

Mohnen, A., Pokorny, K., & Sliwka, D. (2008). Transparency, inequity aversion, and the dynamics of peer pressure in teams: Theory and evidence. Journal of Labor Economics, 26 (4), 693–720.

Perdue, B. C., & Summers, J. O. (1986). Checking the success of manipulations in marketing experiments. Journal of Marketing Research, 23 (4), 317–326.

Robson, C. (2011). Real world research: A resource for users of social research methods in applied settings (3rd ed.). Wiley.

Rogers, W. S. (2003). Social psychology: Experimental and critical approaches . Open University Press.

Sandri, S., Schade, C., Mußhoff, O., & Odening, M. (2010). Holding on for too long? An experimental study on inertia in entrepreneurs’ and non-entrepreneurs’ disinvestment choices. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 76 (1), 30–44.

Schoen, K., & Crilly, N. (2012). Implicit methods for testing product preference: Exploratory studies with the affective simon task. In J. Brasset, J. McDonnell, & M. Malpass (Eds.), Proceedings of 8th international design and emotion conference, ed. London: Central Saint Martins College of Art and Design.

Stier, W. (1999). Empirische Forschungsmethoden . Springer.

Trochim, W. (2005). Research methods: The concise knowledge base. Atomic Dog Pub.

Weber, M., & Zuchel, H. (2005). How do prior outcomes affect risk attitude? Comparing escalation of commitment and the house-money effect. Decision Analysis, 2 (1), 30–43.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Wirtschaft/IFZ – Campus Zug-Rotkreuz, Hochschule Luzern, Zug-Rotkreuz, Zug , Switzerland

Stefan Hunziker & Michael Blankenagel

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stefan Hunziker .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden GmbH, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Hunziker, S., Blankenagel, M. (2021). Experimental Research Design. In: Research Design in Business and Management. Springer Gabler, Wiesbaden. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-34357-6_12

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-34357-6_12

Published : 10 November 2021

Publisher Name : Springer Gabler, Wiesbaden

Print ISBN : 978-3-658-34356-9

Online ISBN : 978-3-658-34357-6

eBook Packages : Business and Economics (German Language)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

Examples

Experimental Research

Ai generator.

experimental research paper sample pdf

Humans are born curious. As babies, we quench our questions by navigating our surroundings with our available senses. Our fascination with the unknown lingers to adulthood that some of us build a career out of trying to discover the mysteries of the universe. To learn about a point of interest, one of the things we do is isolate and replicate the phenomenon in laboratories and controlled environment. Experimental research is a causal investigation into the cause-effect relationships by manipulating all other factors.  You may also see Student Research examples & samples

Experimental research is generally a quantitative research centered on validating or refuting certain claims on causative relationships of matter. We use this method in natural, applied, theoretical, and social sciences, to name a few. This research follows a scientific design that puts a weight on replicability. When the methodology and results are replicable, the study is verifiable by reviewers and critics.

Notable Experiments

We have been conducting experiments for the longest time. Experimental studies done some thousand of years ago prove that unrefined apparatus and limited knowledge, we were already trying to answer the questions of the universe. We had to start somewhere.

Anatomical Anomaly

Even before, societal beliefs have restricted scientific development. This is especially true for modern medicine. Back then, studying and opening cadavers is a punishable crime. Therefore, physicians based their knowledge on the human body on animal dissections. Because animals have a different body organization than humans, this limited what we knew about ourselves. It took actual studies and experiments on the human body to curtail the misinformation and improve medical knowledge.

Reviewing Resemblance

A garden of fuschia and peas helped change our understanding of heredity and inheritable traits. Mendel was curious about why the fuschia plants generate the colors of the flowers the way they do. He crossed varieties of the plant and obtained consistent results. He also tried to cross pea plants and came with repeatable results. The characteristics of the parent plants are passed down to their offsprings to a certain degree of similarity. He also figured out the predictability of certain traits to appear in the offspring. Mendelian genetics explain the laws of inheritance that are still relevant today.

Canine Conditioning 

In the history of psychology research, one experiment will always ring a bell. Pavlov conditioned a dog to expect food when a bell was rung. After repetitions of this approach, the dog started to salivate at the sound of the bell, even when Pavlov didn’t introduce the food. His work on training the reflexes and the mind is in line with the plasticity of the brain to learn and unlearn relationships based on stimuli.

Correlation Vs. Causation

We can opt for an experimental approach to research when we want to determine if the hypothesized cause follows the expected effect. We do this by following a scientific research method and design that emphasizes the replicability of results to limit and reduce biases. By isolating the variables and manipulating treatments, we can establish causation. This is important if we are to find out the relationship between A and B.

In our experiments, we will encounter two or more phenomena, and we might mislabel their connection. There are instances where that relationship is both correlative and causative. What we need to remember is that correlation is not causation. We can say that A causes B when event B is an explicit product of and entirely dependent on event A. Events A and B are correlated when they appear together, but after experimentation, A doesn’t necessarily result in B.

However, it is not enough to say A caused B. Our results are still subject to statistical treatment to determine the validity of the findings and the degree of causation. We still have to ask how much A influences B. Only then can we accept or reject our hypothesis .

Experimental Research Value

Experimental research is a trial-and-error with an educated basis. It lets us determine what works and what doesn’t, and the underlying relationship. In our daily life, we are engaging in pseudo experiments. While cooking, for instance, you taste the dish before you decide to pour additional seasoning. You test first if the food is fine without additives.

In some fields of science, the results of an experiment can be used to generalized a relationship as true for similar, if not all, cases. Experimental research papers make way for the formation of theories. When those theories become unrefuted for a long time, they can become laws that explain universal phenomena.

10+ Experimental Research Examples

Go over the following examples of experimental research papers . They may be able to help you gain a head start in your study or uproot you from where you’re stuck in your experiment.

1. Experimental Research Design Example

Experimental Research Design Example

Size: 465 KB

2. Experimental Data Quality Research Example

Experimental Data Quality Research Example

Size: 318 KB

3. Experimental Research on Labor Market Discrimination

Experimental Research on Labor Market Discrimination

Size: 722 KB

4. Experimental Studies Research Example

Experimental Studies Research

Size: 230 KB

5. Short Description of Experimental Research Example

Short Description of Experimental Research

Size: 280 KB

6. Sample Experimental Design Research Example

Sample Experimental Research

Size: 109 KB

7. Experimental Research on Democracy Example

Experimental Research on Democracy Example

Size: 86 KB

8. Standards for Experimental Research Example

Standards for Experimental Researchs

Size: 141 KB

9. Experimental Research for Evaluation Example

Experimental Research for Evaluation

Size: 87 KB

10. Defense Experimental Research Example

Defense Experimental Research Example

Size: 315 KB

11. Formal Experoimental Research in DOC

Formal Experoimental Research in DOC

How To Start Your Experiment

The best scientists and researchers started with the basics, too. Here are reminders on how you could improve your research writing skills. Who knows, one day, you will join the ranks of world changers with your experimental research report

1. Identify the Problem

To solve a problem, you need to define what it is first. You can begin with identifying the field of research you wish to investigate, then find gaps in knowledge from the related literature. An original work on a timely and relevant issue will help with the approval of your research proposal . After you have read scholarly articles about the topic, you can start narrowing the focus of your research into a specific topic.

2. Design the Experiment

Create a research plan for your intended research with the following notes. The experimental research design ideally employs a probabilistic sampling method to avoid biases from influencing the validity of your work. However, certain experiments call for non-probabilistic sampling techniques. Your experiment should have a control group with ambient conditions or blank treatments. This set up helps you objectively quantify the relationship between A and B.

3. Test the Hypothesis

In performing your experiment, you should have a variable that you would manipulate. The effect of the manipulation will be reflected in the dependent variable. By manipulating the factors that would cause event B, you can determine if A does, in fact, cause B. You can input the raw data into statistical analysis software and tools to see if you can derive a valid conclusion on the relationship between A and B. Correlation or causation and their degree can also be determined by different statistical tests.

4. Publish the Findings

After you have gone through all the efforts in conducting your research, the next step is communicating the findings to the academic community and the public, especially if public and government entities funded the study. You do this by submitting your paper to journals and academic conferences. For what use is the new knowledge you have worked for if you keep the results to yourself?

Experimental research separates science from fiction. Despite criticisms that this method exists in an ideal world, removed from reality, we cannot downsize its merits in the search for knowledge. Because the results are observable, replicable, and appreciable in a real-world sense, this research type will always have room in the development of scientific knowledge and the improvement of man. For as long as man is curious, science will keep growing.

Twitter

Text prompt

  • Instructive
  • Professional

10 Examples of Public speaking

20 Examples of Gas lighting

COMMENTS

  1. (PDF) Exploring Experimental Research: Methodologies, Designs, and

    Experimental research serves as a fundamental scientific method aimed at unraveling cause-and-effect relationships between variables across various disciplines. This paper delineates the key ...

  2. (Pdf) an Experimental Study on The Effect of Parts Strategy and Anxiety

    PDF | The aim of this study was to prove whether the implementation of PARTS Strategy and the students' anxiety gave a significant effect to the... | Find, read and cite all the research you ...

  3. APA Sample Paper: Experimental Psychology

    This material may not be published, reproduced, broadcast, rewritten, or redistributed without permission. Use of this site constitutes acceptance of our terms and conditions of fair use. Media File: APA Sample Paper: Experimental Psychology This resource is enhanced by an Acrobat PDF file. Download the free Acrobat Reader.

  4. (PDF) Experimental Research Design-types & process

    Abstract Experimental research follows strict controls of the researcher. This type of research design is popular in scientific experiments, social sciences, medical science, etc.

  5. PDF An Experimental Study on the Effectiveness of Multimedia

    This paper reports a study that compares the effectiveness of multimedia assisted methods and traditional mode in teaching English to non-English majors. The results show that multimedia methods do not have positive effects on students' English learning, which challenges the common belief and the constructivist theory.

  6. PDF Experimental research to the effects of surface screens on a mobile bed

    Part two of this study is an experimental study on the effects of surface screens on a mobile bed. The design of the physical experiments requires choices about the geometry of the flume. Four different variants have been evaluated using a multicriteria analysis. This analysis gives insight in the choices made.

  7. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General: Sample articles

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. ®. The Role of Political Devotion in Sharing Partisan Misinformation and Resistance to Fact-Checking (PDF, 3.3MB) June 2023. by Clara Pretus, Camila Servin-Barthet, Elizabeth A. Harris, William J. Brady, Oscar Vilarroya, and Jay J. Van Bavel. The Secret to Happiness: Feeling Good or Feeling Right?

  8. PDF Sample Paper: One-Experiment Paper

    A sample paper that follows the APA style guidelines for formatting and citation. The paper reports a study on the effects of age on detection of emotional information and discusses the results and implications.

  9. PDF Experimental Research

    Experimental research may involve the most complicated research designs—that is, until one becomes accustomed to reading it—but it is the only way to obtain a definite answer to the question of why something happens. That is because experimental research is the only way to test causal hypothe-ses directly.

  10. PDF Experimental Research

    Quasi-experimental research and ex post-facto are commonly used in health and social research. More information on research designs with quasi-experimental and ex post-facto are provided in later sections in this chapter.

  11. PDF Experimental Design 1

    Experimental Design. An experiment is "that portion of research in which variables are. served" (Campbell &S. nley, 1963, p. 171). Or stated another way, experime. ts are concerned withan independent variable (IV) that causes or predicts the outcome of the de.

  12. Beauty sleep: experimental study on the perceived health and

    Objective To investigate whether sleep deprived people are perceived as less healthy, less attractive, and more tired than after a normal night's sleep. Design Experimental study. Setting Sleep laboratory in Stockholm, Sweden. Participants 23 healthy, sleep deprived adults (age 18-31) who were photographed and 65 untrained observers (age 18-61) who rated the photographs. Intervention ...

  13. Exploring Experimental Research: Methodologies, Designs, and ...

    Through a comprehensive examination of experimental research methodologies, designs, and applications, this paper aims to provide researchers with a nuanced understanding of experimental inquiry across diverse academic domains.

  14. (PDF) Experimental Research Methods

    PDF | On Jan 1, 2003, S.M. Ross and others published Experimental Research Methods | Find, read and cite all the research you need on ResearchGate

  15. (PDF) EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH METHODS

    Hence, it is crucial to use multi-method research approaches (employing the experimental method as appropriate, depending on the research problem) in preparing (and approving) doctoral dissertations, writing reviews and publishing research papers. Download Free PDF View PDF Mind, Brain, and Education Melina Uncapher Download Free PDF View PDF ...

  16. PDF Quantitative Research Designs: Experimental, Quasi-Experimental, and

    Then they use a traditional research design (obviously not experimental or quasi-experimental) to answer a research question. For example, Eicher-Miller, Khanna, Boushey, Gelfand, and Delp (2016) used data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (1999-2004) to test a hypothesis.

  17. PDF 12 Experimental Research Design

    12.3 Writing an Experimental Research Paper. Writing an experimental research paper follows the principles and structure detailed in Chap. 4. However, there are some aspects especially important for reports about experi-mental research projects or (partly) different from reports about other research projects.

  18. PDF 9781284048308_CH05_059_076.indd

    The primary difference between the true experiment and quasi-experimental designs is the degree of control that the researcher has over the subjects and variables of the study. Control is much easier to achieve in the laboratory than in the field. In nursing research, the "field" includes homes, hospitals, clinics, schools, the workplace, or wherever we find people with health concerns ...

  19. (PDF) An Introduction to Experimental Design Research

    Thus, this book brings together leading researchers from across design research in order to provide the reader with a foundation in experimental design research; an appreciation of possible ...

  20. Experimental Research

    10+ Experimental Research Examples Go over the following examples of experimental research papers. They may be able to help you gain a head start in your study or uproot you from where you're stuck in your experiment.

  21. PDF Experimental Research Methods Environment and Engineering

    This paper describes the deductive approach to research in engineering and built environment and focuses mainly on the use of experiments for data gathering. Details of experimental methods in the laboratory, field and numerical conditions are discussed. Various sensors and transducers commonly used in experiments as well as data logging ...

  22. (PDF) Experimental research

    This paper outlines the evolution of experi mental research, makes a characterization of the experiment, describes th e experimental research process and highlights the importance of considering ...