This Huge Study Is The Best Evidence Yet That Your Personality Changes Over Time

This huge study is the best evidence yet that your personality changes over time

You might be fundamentally you for your entire life, but don't expect your personality to stay the same.

That's according to a major study of 50,000 people over the course of several decades, which found the traditional notion of personality – as fixed and unchanged after adolescence – is mostly untrue.

People included in the sample showed a common trend as they got older, declining in all five major personality metrics that psychologists have come to trust as the gold-standard.

Psychologists have been writing about personality for the better part of three centuries, beginning most famously with William James' 1890 text The Principles of Psychology . Relying on personal observation, James wrote that personality is "set like plaster" after age 30.

In the century or so since The Principles of Psychology was published, psychologists have come to rethink personality in bits and pieces.

In 2003, the American Psychological Association observed the changing consensus among members of the field: Personality was beginning to look more like it was ever-evolving, even through old age.

The latest study combined 14 longitudinal studies that gathered information about people's personalities, including data from the United States, Europe, and Scandinavia.

Many of the subjects had already reached adulthood, which gave the researchers unique perspective on personality changes. Typically, studies skew toward young people.

Of the Big Five personality traits – neuroticism, conscientiousness, openness, extroversion, and agreeableness – all five showed major fluctuations across individual participants' lives.

And all traits, except for agreeableness, showed downward trends of about 1-2 percent per decade across the overall studies.

In part, this suggested to researchers that the so-called "Dolce Vita" effect was real – that when people age, they enjoy fewer social responsibilities and can do more of what they want.

People can be less neurotic about conforming to the group, less open to trying new things in order to savour the classics, less conscientious as they become more selfish, and less extroverted as they keep more to themselves.

These trends appeared at nearly every stage of the 14 studies and held mostly steady across different geographical regions. Some regions deviated from the norm, however.

People from the US showed considerably larger declines in extroversion as they aged, which signalled to the investigators that they were especially done with with seeming social.

People aren't set in plaster, as William James asserted 128 years ago. They're more like clay, constantly getting moulded by their changing circumstances.

This article was originally published by Business Insider .

Score Card Research NoScript

More From Forbes

New research identifies personality types that are naturally good at self-care.

  • Share to Facebook
  • Share to Twitter
  • Share to Linkedin

Extraverts have the upper hand when it comes to caring for their basic psychological needs.

A new study published in the Journal of Research in Personality identifies the types of people who may be best equipped to satisfy their basic psychological needs, such as the need for autonomy, competence, and relatedness.

The researchers, led by Martina Pocrnic of the University of Zagreb in Croatia, found the traits of extraversion, conscientiousness, and agreeableness to be most strongly associated with self-care behaviors and routines — and that people who possessed these traits were most likely to care for their basic needs.

“Although basic psychological needs are a human universal, there are individual differences in the degree to which people satisfy those needs,” comments Pocrnic. “We wanted to investigate how individual differences in the satisfaction of basic psychological needs arise and how they are related to differences in personality.”

To study this, the researchers asked 668 Croatian adults to complete a series of psychological questionnaires that measured various dimensions of need satisfaction and personality. Specifically, they asked respondents to self-report on the following dimensions of psychological needs:

  • Sense of competence — i.e., feeling effective and having opportunities to express and expand one’s abilities
  • Sense of autonomy — i.e., having the ability to make one’s own choices in life
  • Sense of relatedness — i.e., feeling connected and sharing a sense of belonging with others

Best Travel Insurance Companies

Best covid-19 travel insurance plans.

The researchers used the “Big Five” model of personality, which divides personality into five distinct dimensions (agreeableness, neuroticism, conscientiousness, openness, and extraversion), to measure respondents’ personality traits.

They found extraversion and neuroticism to be most predictive of the three dimensions of psychological need satisfaction. Specifically, higher levels of extraversion promoted higher levels of need satisfaction while higher levels of neuroticism promoted lower levels of need satisfaction.

“It was a surprise to us that neuroticism and extraversion had the biggest correlations with all three needs — autonomy, competence, and relatedness,” says Pocrnic. “We had expected that all three needs would show significant relationships with at least one trait, but we had not expected that extraversion and neuroticism would be the crucial traits for all three needs. That was certainly the most interesting finding for us.”

They also found conscientiousness to be important when satisfying the need for competence and agreeableness to play a role in satisfying the need for autonomy and relatedness.

Pocrnic offers the following example to explain how personality traits impact our ability to satisfy our basic psychological needs.

“Someone with a high level of extraversion is likely to have a wide social network, is more prone to initiate contact with other people, spend leisure time socializing, and accept invitations to social events,” says Pocrnic. “All of this can lead to situations that satisfy the need for relatedness. Another example is a student who has a high level of conscientiousness. Because of that personality trait, he/she is hardworking, organized, and highly self-disciplined and likely to study regularly, achieve better grades, and consequently satisfy the need for competence.”

Importantly, the authors point out that personality traits are not fixed and that everyone has an opportunity to better meet their basic psychological needs.

“Although personality traits by definition are relatively stable over time, they are not set in stone,” says Pocrnic. “If we try to lower our levels of neuroticism through practicing meditation or mindfulness, for example, it could lead to greater satisfaction of our psychological needs.”

In the future, the authors hope to examine how a more nuanced analysis of personality might influence their results.

“It would be interesting to find out which specific facets of neuroticism are more important for low levels of need satisfaction,” says Pocrnic. “Is it anxiety, depression, anger, or perhaps vulnerability?”

A full interview with Martina Pocrnic discussing her new research can be found here: Which personality traits facilitate self-care?

Mark Travers

  • Editorial Standards
  • Reprints & Permissions

July 1, 2009

What Your Choice of Words Says about Your Personality

A language analysis program reveals personality, mental health and intent by counting and categorizing words

By Jan Dönges

NO ONE DOUBTS that the words we write or speak are an expression of our inner thoughts and personalities. But beyond the meaningful content of language, a wealth of unique insights into an author’s mind are hidden in the style of a text—in such elements as how often certain words and word categories are used, regardless of context.

It is how an author expresses his or her thoughts that reveals character, asserts social psychologist James W. Pennebaker of the University of Texas at Austin. When people try to present themselves a certain way, they tend to select what they think are appropriate nouns and verbs, but they are unlikely to control their use of articles and pronouns. These small words create the style of a text, which is less subject to conscious manipulation.

Pennebaker’s statistical analyses have shown that these small words may hint at the healing progress of patients and give us insight into the personalities and changing ideals of public figures, from political candidates to terrorists. “Virtually no one in psychology has realized that low-level words can give clues to large-scale behaviors,” says Pennebaker, who, with colleagues, developed a computer program that analyzes text, called Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC, pronounced “Luke”). The software has been used to examine other speech characteristics as well, tallying up nouns and verbs in hundreds of categories to expose buried patterns.

On supporting science journalism

If you're enjoying this article, consider supporting our award-winning journalism by subscribing . By purchasing a subscription you are helping to ensure the future of impactful stories about the discoveries and ideas shaping our world today.

Character Count Most recently, Pennebaker and his colleagues used LIWC to analyze the candidates’ speeches and interviews during last fall’s presidential election. The software counts how many times a speaker or author uses words in specific categories, such as emotion or perception, and words that indicate complex cognitive processes. It also tallies up so-called function words such as pronouns, articles, numerals and conjunctions. Within each of these major categories are subsets: Are there more mentions of sad or happy emotions? Does the speaker prefer “I” and “me” to “us” and “we”? LIWC answers these quantitative questions; psychologists must then figure out what the numbers mean. Before LIWC was developed in the mid-1990s, years of psychological research in which people counted words by hand established robust connections between word usage and psychological states or character traits

The political candidates, for example, showed clear differences in their speaking styles. John McCain tended to speak directly and personally to his constituency, using a vocabulary that was both emotionally loaded and impulsive. Barack Obama, in contrast, made frequent use of causal relationships, which indicated more complex thought processes. He also tended to be more vague than his Republican rival. Pennebaker’s team has posted a far more in-depth breakdown, including analyses of the vice presidential candidates, at www.wordwatchers.wordpress.com .

Skeptics of LIWC’s usefulness point out that many of these characteristics of McCain’s and Obama’s speeches could be gleaned without the use of a computer program. When the subjects of analysis are not accessible, however, LIWC may provide a unique insight. Such was the case with Pennebaker’s study of al Qaeda communications. In 2007 he and several co-workers, under contract with the FBI, analyzed 58 texts by Osama bin Laden and Ayman al-Zawahiri, bin Laden’s second in command.

The comparison showed how much pronouns are able to disclose. For example, between 2004 and 2006 the frequency with which al-Zawahiri used the word “I” tripled, whereas it remained constant in bin Laden’s writings. “Normally, higher rates of ‘I’ words correspond with feelings of insecurity, threat and defensiveness. Closer inspection of his ‘I’ use in context tends to confirm this,” Pennebaker says.

Other studies have shown that words that are used to express balance or nuance (“except,” “but,” and so on) are associated with higher cognitive complexity, better grades and even the truthfulness with which facts are reported. For bin Laden, analysis showed that the thought processes in his texts had reached a higher level over the years, whereas those of his lieutenant had stagnated.

Healing Words This power of statistical analysis to quantify a person’s changing language use over time is a key advantage to programs such as LIWC. In 2003 Pennebaker and statistician R. Sherlock Campbell, now at Yale University, used a statistical tool called latent semantic analysis (LSA) to study the diary entries of trauma patients from three earlier studies, looking for text characteristics that had changed in patients who were convalescing and met rarely with their physician. Again, the researchers showed that content was unimportant. The factor that was most clearly associated with recovery was the use of pronouns. Patients whose writings changed perspective from day to day were less likely to seek medical treatment during the follow-up period.

It may be that patients who describe their situation both from their own viewpoint and from the perspective of others recover more quickly from traumatic experiences—a variation on the already well-established idea that writing about negative experiences is therapeutic. Or perhaps the LSA simply detected the patients’ recovery as reflected by their writing but not brought about by it—in that case, programs such as LIWC could aid doctors in diagnosing illness and gauging treatment progression. Researchers are currently investigating many other patient groups, including those with cancer, mental illness and suicidal tendencies, using LIWC to uncover clues about their emotional well-being and their mental state.

Although the statistical study of language is relatively young, it is clear that analyzing patterns of word use and writing style can lead to insights that would otherwise remain hidden. Because these tools offer predictions based on probability, however, such insights will never be definitive. “In the final analysis, our situation is much like that of economists,” Pennebaker says. “It’s too early to come up with a standardized analysis. But at the end of the day, we all are making educated guesses, the same way economists can understand, explain and predict economic ups and downs.”

He Said, She Said The way we write and speak can reveal volumes about our identity and character. Here is a sampling of the many variables that can be detected in our use of style-related words such as pronouns and articles:

Gender : In general, women tend to use more pronouns and references to other people. Men are more likely to use articles, prepositions and big words.

Age : As people get older, they typically refer to themselves less, use more positive-emotion words and fewer negative-emotion words, and use more future-tense verbs and fewer past-tense verbs.

Honesty : When telling the truth, people are more likely to use first-person singular pronouns such as “I.” They also use exclusive words such as “except” and “but.” These words may indicate that a person is making a distinction between what they did do and what they did not do—liars often do not deal well with such complex constructions.

Depression and suicide risk : Public figures and published poets use more first-person singular pronouns when they are depressed or suicidal, possibly indicating excessive self-absorption and social isolation.

Reaction to trauma : In the days and weeks after a cultural upheaval, people use “I” less and “we” more, suggesting a social bonding effect.

Note: This article was originally printed with the title, "You Are What You Say."

Jan Dönges is an editor at Spektrum der Wissenschaft .

SA Mind Vol 20 Issue 4

Personality Assessments: 10 Best Inventories, Tests, & Methods

Personality Assessments

Perhaps they respond differently to news or react differently to your feedback. They voice different opinions and values and, as such, behave differently.

If you respond with a resounding yes, we understand the challenges you face.

As more and more organizations diversify their talent, a new challenge emerges of how to get the best out of employees and teams of all personality configurations.

In this article, we embark on a whistle-stop tour of the science of personality, focusing on personality assessments to measure clients’ and employees’ character plus the benefits of doing so, before rounding off with practical tools for those who want to bolster their professional toolkits.

Before you continue, we thought you might like to download our three Strengths Exercises for free . These detailed, science-based exercises will help your clients or employees realize their unique potential and create a life that feels energizing and authentic.

This Article Contains

What are personality assessments in psychology, 4 methods and types of personality assessments, 7 evidence-based inventories, scales, and tests.

  • Helpful Tools & Questions

Fascinating Books About Personality Assessments

Resources from positivepsychology.com, a take-home message.

Personality is a tricky concept to define in concrete terms, and this is reflected both in the number of personality theories that exist and the lack of consensus among personality psychologists.

However, for this article, we can think of personality as the totality of one’s behavioral patterns and subjective experiences (Kernberg, 2016).

All individuals have a constellation of traits and experiences that make them unique yet simultaneously suggest that there are some generalizable or distinct qualities inherent in all humans.

In psychology, we are interested in understanding how traits and qualities that people possess cluster together and the extent to which these vary across and within individuals.

Now, it’s all very well and good knowing that personality exists as a concept and that your employees and clients differ in their groupings of traits and subjective experiences, but how can you apply this information to your professional work with them?

This is where measuring and assessing personality comes into play. Like most psychological concepts, researchers want to show that theoretical knowledge can be useful for working life and brought to bear in the real world.

For example, knowing a client’s or employee’s personality can be key to setting them up for success at work and pursuing and achieving work-related goals. But we first need to identify or assess personality before we can help others to reap these benefits.

Personality assessments are used for several reasons.

First, they can provide professionals with an opportunity to identify their strengths and reaffirm their sense of self. It is no coincidence that research on strengths is so popular or that strengths have such a prominent place in the working world. People like to know who they are, and they want to capitalize on the qualities and traits they possess.

Second, personality assessments can provide professionals with a social advantage by helping them to understand how they are perceived by others such as colleagues, managers, and stakeholders — the looking glass self (Cooley, 1902).

In the sections below, we will explore different personality assessments and popular evidence-based scales.

Personality types

1. Self-report assessments

Self-reports are one of the most widely used formats for psychometric testing. They are as they sound: reports or questionnaires that a client or employee completes themselves (and often scores themselves).

Self-report measures can come in many formats. The most common are Likert scales where individuals are asked to rate numerically (from 1 to 7 for example) the extent to which they feel that each question describes their thoughts, feelings, or behaviors.

These types of assessments are popular because they are easy to distribute and complete, they are often cost effective, and they can provide helpful insights into behavior. Self-reports can be completed in both personal and professional settings and can be particularly helpful in a coaching practice, for example.

However, they also have downsides to be wary of, including an increase in unconscious biases such as the social desirability bias (i.e., the desire to answer “correctly”). They can also be prone to individuals not paying attention, not answering truthfully, or not fully understanding the questions asked.

Such issues can lead to an inaccurate assessment of personality.

However, if you are a professional working with clients in any capacity, it is advised to first try out any self-report measure before suggesting them to clients. In this way, you can gauge for yourself the usefulness and validity of the measure.

2. Behavioral observation

Another useful method of personality assessment is behavioral observation. This method entails someone observing and documenting a person’s behavior.

While this method is more resource heavy in terms of time and requires an observer (preferably one who is experienced and qualified in observing and coding the behavior), it can be useful as a complementary method employed alongside self-reports because it can provide an external corroboration of behavior.

Alternatively, behavioral observation can fail to corroborate self-report scores, raising the question of how reliably an individual has answered their self-report.

3. Interviews

Interviews are used widely from clinical settings to workplaces to determine an individual’s personality. Even a job interview is a test of behavioral patterns and experiences (i.e., personality).

During such interviews, the primary aim is to gather as much information as possible by using probing questions. Responses should be recorded, and there should be a standardized scoring system to determine the outcome of the interview (for example, whether the candidate is suitable for the role).

While interviews can elicit rich data about a client or employee, they are also subject to the unconscious biases of the interviewers and can be open to interpretation if there is no method for scoring or evaluating the interviewee.

4. Projective tests

These types of tests are unusual in that they present individuals with an abstract or vague object, task, or activity and require them to describe what they see. The idea here is that the unfiltered interpretation can provide insight into the person’s psychology and way of thinking.

A well-known example of a projective test is the Rorschach inkblot test. However, there are limitations to projective tests due to their interpretative nature and the lack of a consistent or quantifiable way of coding or scoring individuals’ responses.

research on personality reveals that it is

Download 3 Free Strengths Exercises (PDF)

These detailed, science-based exercises will equip you or your clients with tools to discover and harness their unique strengths.

Download 3 Free Strengths Tools Pack (PDF)

By filling out your name and email address below.

Personality assessments can be used in the workplace during recruitment to gauge whether someone would be a good fit for a job or organization and to help determine job performance, career progression, and development.

Below, we highlight a few commonly used inventories and tests for such career assessments.

1. The Hogan personality inventory (HPI)

The Hogan personality inventory (Hogan & Hogan, 2002) is a self-report personality assessment created by Robert Hogan and Joyce Hogan in the late 1970s.

It was originally based on the California Personality Inventory (Gough, 1975) and also draws upon the five-factor model of personality. The five-factor model of personality suggests there are five key dimensions of personality: openness to experience , conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism (Digman, 1990).

The Hogan assessment comprises 206 items across seven different scales that measure and predict social behavior and social outcomes rather than traits or qualities , as do other popular personality measures.

These seven scales include:

  • Sociability
  • Interpersonal sensitivity
  • Inquisitiveness
  • Learning approach

The HPI’s primary use is within organizations to help with recruitment and the development of leaders. It is a robust scale with over 40 years of evidence to support it, and the scale itself takes roughly 15–20 minutes to complete (Hogan Assessments, n.d.).

2. DISC test

The DISC test of personality developed by Merenda and Clarke (1965) is a very popular personality self-assessment used primarily within the corporate world. It is based on the emotional and behavioral DISC theory (Marston, 1928), which measures individuals on four dimensions of behavior:

The self-report comprises 24 questions and takes roughly 10 minutes to complete. While the test is simpler and quicker to complete than other popular tests (e.g., the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator), it has been subject to criticism regarding its psychometric properties.

3. Gallup – CliftonStrengths™ Assessment

Unlike the DISC test, the CliftonStrengths™ assessment , employed by Gallup and based on the work of Marcus Buckingham and Don Clifton (2001), is a questionnaire designed specifically to help individuals identify strengths in the workplace and learn how to use them.

The assessment is a self-report Likert scale comprising 177 questions and takes roughly 30 minutes to complete. Once scored, the assessment provides individuals with 34 strength themes organized into four key domains:

  • Strategic thinking
  • Influencing
  • Relationship building

The scale has a solid theoretical and empirical grounding, making it a popular workplace assessment around the world.

4. NEO-PI-R

The NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 2008) is a highly popular self-report personality assessment based on Allport and Odbert’s (1936) trait theory of personality.

With good reliability, this scale has amassed a large evidence base, making it an appealing inventory for many. The NEO-PI-R assesses an individual’s strengths, talents, and weaknesses and is often used by employers to identify suitable candidates for job openings.

It uses the big five factors of personality (openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism) and also includes an additional six subcategories within the big five, providing a detailed breakdown of each personality dimension.

The scale itself comprises 240 questions that describe different behaviors and takes roughly 30–40 minutes to complete. Interestingly, this inventory can be administered as a self-report or, alternatively, as an observational report, making it a favored assessment among professionals.

5. Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ)

The EPQ is a personality assessment developed by personality psychologists Hans Eysenck and Sybil Eysenck (1975).

The scale results from successive revisions and improvements of earlier scales: the Maudsley Personality Inventory (Eysenck 1959) and Eysenck Personality Inventory (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1964).

The aim of the EPQ is to measure the three dimensions of personality as espoused by Eysenck’s psychoticism–extraversion–neuroticism theory of personality The scale itself uses a Likert format and was revised and shortened in 1992 to include 48 items (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1992).

This is a generally useful scale; however, some researchers have found that there are reliability issues with the psychoticism subscale, likely because this was a later addition to the scale.

6. Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI)

The MMPI (Hathaway & McKinley, 1943) is one of the most widely used personality inventories in the world and uses a true/false format of questioning.

It was initially designed to assess mental health problems in clinical settings during the 1940s and uses 10 clinical subscales to assess different psychological conditions.

The inventory was revised in the 1980s, resulting in the MMPI-2, which comprised 567 questions, and again in 2020, resulting in the MMPI-3, which comprises a streamlined 338 questions.

While the revised MMPI-3 takes a lengthy 35–50 minutes to complete, it remains popular to this day, particularly in clinical settings, and enables the accurate capture of aspects of psychopathy and mental health disturbance. The test has good reliability but must be administered by a professional.

7. 16 Personality Factor Questionnaire (16PF)

The 16PF (Cattell et al., 1970) is another rating scale inventory used primarily in clinical settings to identify psychiatric disorders by measuring “normal” personality traits.

Cattell identified 16 primary personality traits, with five secondary or global traits underneath that map onto the big five factors of personality.

These include such traits as warmth, reasoning, and emotional stability, to name a few. The most recent version of the questionnaire (the fifth edition) comprises 185 multiple-choice questions that ask about routine behaviors on a 10-point scale and takes roughly 35–50 minutes to complete.

The scale is easy to administer and well validated but must be administered by a professional.

research on personality reveals that it is

World’s Largest Positive Psychology Resource

The Positive Psychology Toolkit© is a groundbreaking practitioner resource containing over 500 science-based exercises , activities, interventions, questionnaires, and assessments created by experts using the latest positive psychology research.

Updated monthly. 100% Science-based.

“The best positive psychology resource out there!” — Emiliya Zhivotovskaya , Flourishing Center CEO

Helpful Tools & Questions

In addition to the collection of science-based interventions, we also have to mention a controversial but well-known personality assessment tool: Myers-Briggs.

We share two informative videos on this topic and then move on to a short collection of questions that can be used for career development.

1. Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI)

Personality inventories

A mother and daughter team developed the MBTI in the 1940s during the Second World War. The MBTI comprises 93 questions that aim to measure an individual on four different dimensions of personality:

  • Introversion/extraversion
  • Sensing/ intuition
  • Thinking/feeling
  • Judging/perceiving

The test provides individuals with a type of personality out of a possible 16 combinations. Whilst this test is a favorite in workplaces, there are serious criticisms leveled at how the scale was developed and the lack of rigorous evidence to support its use.

For more information on the MBTI, you might enjoy the below videos:

We recommend that if you employ MBTI, be mindful of its scientific deficiencies and support your personality testing further by completing an additional validated scale.

10 Career development questions

  • Tell me about what inspires you. What gets you out of bed in the morning?
  • Tell me about your vision for your career/life.
  • What aspects of your role do you love? What aspects do you struggle with?
  • Tell me about a time where you used your strengths to achieve a positive outcome.
  • Are there any healthy habits you want to build into your work life?
  • Describe your perfect working day. What would it look like?
  • Tell me about your fears.
  • What do you value most about your job?
  • What goals are you currently working toward?
  • How would your work colleagues describe you?

If you are interested in learning more about personality and personality assessments, the following three books are an excellent place to start.

These books were chosen because they give an excellent overview of what personality is and how it can be measured. They also illuminate some issues with personality assessments. They provide a good grounding for any professional looking to implement personality assessments in the workplace.

1. Mindset: Changing the Way You Think to Fulfil Your Potential – Carol Dweck

Mindset

Enter Dr. Carol Dweck and several decades of psychological research she has conducted on motivation and personality.

The main thesis of the book is to explore the idea that people can have either a fixed or growth mindset (i.e., beliefs we hold about ourselves and the world around us). Adopting a growth mindset can be a critical determinant of outcomes such as performance and academic success.

Find the book on Amazon .

2. The Personality Brokers: The Strange History of Myers-Briggs and the Birth of Personality Testing – Merve Emre

The Personality Brokers

If you are interested in the dark side of psychology assessments, this is the book for you.

This book explores how the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator was developed and discusses the questionable validity of the scale despite its widespread popularity in the corporate world.

While many assessments can be helpful for self-reflecting on your own behavior, The Personality Brokers delve into the murky side of how psychological concepts can be used for monetary gains, even when evidence is lacking or disputed.

3. Psychological Types – Carl Jung

Psychological Types

This is an excellent book from one of history’s most influential psychologists: Carl Jung.

The book focuses most on extraversion and introversion as the two key types of personality and also discusses the limitations of categorizing individuals into “types” of personality.

For those interested in the science of personality and who prefer a slightly heavier, academic read, this book is for you.

Interested in supplementing your professional life by exploring personality types? Here at PositivePsychology.com, we have several highly useful resources.

Maximizing Strengths Masterclass©

While strengths finding is a distinct and popular topic within positive psychology, we can draw parallels between strengths research and some conceptualizations of personality.

The Maximizing Strengths Masterclass© is designed to help clients reach their potential by looking at their strengths and what energizes them and helping them delve into their authentic selves. As a six-module coaching package, it includes 19 videos, a practitioner handbook, slide presentations, and much more.

Recommended Reading

For more information on personality psychology and personality assessments, check out the following related articles.

  • Big Five Personality Traits: The OCEAN Model Explained
  • Personality & Character Traits: The Good, the Bad, the Ugly
  • Personal Strengths Defined (+ List of 92 Personal Strengths)

17 Career exercises

Designed to help people use their personality and strengths at work, this collection of 17 work and career coaching exercises is grounded in scientific evidence. The exercises help individuals and clients identify areas for career growth and development. Some of these exercises include:

  • Achievement Story Chart your successes at work, take time to reflect on your achievements, and identify how to use your strengths for growth.
  • Job Analysis Through a Strengths Lens Identify your strengths and opportunities to use them when encountering challenges at work.
  • Job Satisfaction Wheel Complete the job satisfaction wheel, which measures your current levels of happiness at work across seven different dimensions.
  • What Work Means to You Identify how meaningful your work is to you by assessing your motivational orientation toward work (i.e., whether it is something you are called to and that aligns with your sense of self).

If you’re looking for more science-based ways to help others develop their strengths, this collection contains 17 strength-finding tools for practitioners. Use them to help others better understand and harness their strengths in life-enhancing ways.

research on personality reveals that it is

17 Exercises To Discover & Unlock Strengths

Use these 17 Strength-Finding Exercises [PDF] to help others discover and leverage their unique strengths in life, promoting enhanced performance and flourishing.

Created by Experts. 100% Science-based.

When managing people, it is always helpful to have insight into why they behave the way they do. The same applies to assisting someone on their career path. Having an understanding of the qualities that influence behavioral responses can improve relationships, parenting, how people work, and even goal setting.

But there are some caveats to be mindful of:

  • When using self-reports, take the scores with a pinch of salt, particularly as we all operate with unconscious biases that can skew results.
  • Remain open minded about our personality traits; if we are resigned to the idea that they are inherited at birth, fixed, and unchanging, we are unlikely to gain any real discernment into our own evolving identity.
  • Labels can oftentimes be limiting. Trying to condense the myriad aspects of an individual into a neat “personality” category could backfire.

In the right hands, validated personality assessments are valuable tools for guiding clients on the right career path, ensuring a good job fit and building strong teams.

We hope you enjoyed reading this article. Don’t forget to download our three Strengths Exercises for free .

  • Allport, G. W., & Odbert, H. S. (1936). Trait-names: A psycho-lexical study. Psychological Monographs , 47 (1), i–171.
  • Buckingham, M., & Clifton, D. O. (2001). Now, discover your strengths . Simon and Schuster.
  • Cattell, R. B., Eber, H. W., & Tatsuoka, M. M. (1970). Handbook for the Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire . Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.
  • Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human nature and the social order . Transaction.
  • Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (2008). The Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) . In G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of personality theory and assessment, Vol. 2. Personality measurement and testing (pp. 179–198). SAGE.
  • Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology , 41 (1), 417–440.
  • Eysenck, H. J. (1959). Manual of the Maudsley Personality Inventory . University of London Press.
  • Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1964). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Inventory . University of London Press.
  • Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire . Educational and Industrial Testing Service.
  • Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1992). Manual for the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire–Revised . Educational and Industrial Testing Service.
  • Gough, H. G. (1975). Manual: The California Psychological Inventory (Rev. ed.). Consulting Psychologist Press.
  • Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1943). The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (Rev. ed., 2nd printing). University of Minnesota Press.
  • Hogan Assessments. (n.d.). About. Retrieved May 8, 2023, from https://www.hoganassessments.com/about/.
  • Hogan, R., & Hogan, J. (2002). The Hogan personality inventory. In B. de Raad & M. Perugini (Eds.), Big five assessment (pp. 329–346). Hogrefe & Huber.
  • Kernberg, O. F. (2016). What is personality? Journal of Personality Disorders , 30 (2), 145–156.
  • Marston, W. M. (1928). Emotions of normal people . Kegan Paul Trench Trubner and Company.
  • Merenda, P. F., & Clarke, W. V. (1965). Self description and personality measurement. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21 , 52–56.
  • Myers, I. B., & McCaulley, M. H. (1985). Manual: A guide to the development and use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator . Palo Alto Consulting Psychologists Press.

' src=

Share this article:

Article feedback

What our readers think.

Mariana

Very insightful yet easy to read article, thank you for sharing! Have you heard of the Strength Finder test from Personality Quizzes? https://www.personality-quizzes.com/strength-finder It’s a free version of Clifton Strengths (although you have to pay to see complete results). I liked the experience, may be worth updating the list!

Braham Sharoha

I learned so much. This article gave me more food for thought.

Let us know your thoughts Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published.

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

Related articles

Type B Personality

Type B Personality Advantages: Stress Less, Achieve More

Type B personalities, known for their relaxed, patient, and easygoing nature, offer unique advantages in both personal and professional contexts. There are myriad benefits to [...]

Jungian Psychology

Jungian Psychology: Unraveling the Unconscious Mind

Alongside Sigmund Freud, the Swiss psychiatrist and psychoanalyst Carl Gustav Jung (1875–1961) is one of the most important innovators in the field of modern depth [...]

Jungian Archetypes

12 Jungian Archetypes: The Foundation of Personality

In the vast tapestry of human existence, woven with the threads of individual experiences and collective consciousness, lies a profound understanding of the human psyche. [...]

Read other articles by their category

  • Body & Brain (52)
  • Coaching & Application (39)
  • Compassion (23)
  • Counseling (40)
  • Emotional Intelligence (21)
  • Gratitude (18)
  • Grief & Bereavement (18)
  • Happiness & SWB (40)
  • Meaning & Values (26)
  • Meditation (16)
  • Mindfulness (40)
  • Motivation & Goals (41)
  • Optimism & Mindset (29)
  • Positive CBT (28)
  • Positive Communication (23)
  • Positive Education (36)
  • Positive Emotions (32)
  • Positive Leadership (16)
  • Positive Parenting (14)
  • Positive Psychology (21)
  • Positive Workplace (35)
  • Productivity (16)
  • Relationships (46)
  • Resilience & Coping (38)
  • Self Awareness (20)
  • Self Esteem (37)
  • Strengths & Virtues (29)
  • Stress & Burnout Prevention (33)
  • Theory & Books (42)
  • Therapy Exercises (37)
  • Types of Therapy (54)

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Open access
  • Published: 02 September 2024

The role of personality traits and online behavior in belief in fake news

  • Erika L. Peter   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-7063-3928 1 ,
  • Peter J. Kwantes 1 ,
  • Madeleine T. D’Agata 1 &
  • Janani Vallikanthan 1  

Humanities and Social Sciences Communications volume  11 , Article number:  1126 ( 2024 ) Cite this article

Metrics details

  • Science, technology and society

The current study examines how careless online behavior and personality traits are related to the detection of fake news. We tested the relationships among accurately distinguishing between fake and real news headlines, careless online behavioral tendencies, and the HEXACO and dark triad personality traits. Poorer discernment between fake and real news headlines was associated with greater careless behavior online (i.e., greater online disinhibition, greater risky online behavior, greater engagement with strangers online, and less suspicion of others’ intentions online), as well as lower Conscientiousness, Openness, and Honesty-Humility, and greater dark triad traits. Implications for the literature as well as potential interventions to reduce susceptibility to misinformation and fake news are discussed.

Similar content being viewed by others

research on personality reveals that it is

A prosocial fake news intervention with durable effects

research on personality reveals that it is

The relation between authoritarian leadership and belief in fake news

research on personality reveals that it is

The fingerprints of misinformation: how deceptive content differs from reliable sources in terms of cognitive effort and appeal to emotions

Introduction.

The information environment is leveraged by adversaries, criminals, and individuals to spread fake news, conspiracy theories, disinformation, and misinformation. Footnote 1 Belief in conspiracy theories can increase during times of uncertainty, often to explain things that are out of one’s control (Douglas, 2021 ; Miller, 2020 ; van Prooijen and Acker, 2015 ). Amidst the global COVID-19 pandemic, for example, conspiracy theories and misinformation about the spread, prevention, and severity of the disease proliferated (e.g., Douglas, 2021 ). Research shows that conspiratorial beliefs regarding medicine and health can have behavioral consequences. For example, holding conspiratorial beliefs about AIDS has been associated with reduced safer sex practices (Grebe and Nattrass, 2012 ). In addition, holding conspiratorial beliefs about birth control being a form of Black genocide reduced contraceptive use in African American respondents (Thorburn and Bogart, 2005 ). Further still, individuals who held more anti-vaccine conspiracy beliefs, and those who were exposed to anti-vaccine conspiracy theories, were less likely to report intentions to vaccinate their children than those who did not (Jolley and Douglas, 2014 ). From the examples above, the uptake of false information can have real consequences on participation in public health measures like vaccine uptake, which has been a priority since COVID-19 vaccines became available. Endorsement of conspiracy theories about COVID-19 have even been associated with self-reported reduced participation, compliance (Earnshaw et al., 2020 ; Pummerer et al., 2022 ), and support for public health measures like handwashing, social distancing (Allington et al., 2020 ; Bierwiaczonek et al., 2020 ), mask-wearing (Romer and Jamieson, 2020 ), and intentions to become vaccinated (Earnshaw et al., 2020 ; Romer and Jamieson, 2020 ). In sum, there seems to exist a portion of the population that possesses a propensity to believe, or at least hold plausible, conspiracy theories that propagate throughout the information environment, and in response, adjust their behavior in a manner accordingly. In the following sections, we explore current work done to understand the personality factors that have been identified as diagnostic indicators of a person’s propensity to believe untrue or potentially misleading information and propose an expansion of the role of personality by including the socially aversive so-called ‘dark triad’ traits that have been examined in the literature.

The role of personality

Intuitively, whether a person is influenced by misinformation in the information environment depends on their ability to identify it as misinformation in the first place. But to what extent might personality make one more or less able to do so? While personality is traditionally assessed using the Big Five model of personality (John et al., 1991 ), for this article, we used a framework of personality that includes a sixth trait, Honesty-Humility (Ashton et al., 2014 ; Ashton and Lee, 2007 ), with the remaining five traits sharing much similarity with the Big Five. Together, they are (along with their behavioral and emotional tendencies in parentheses): Honesty-Humility (honest, sincere, fair, modest), Emotionality (vulnerable, sensitive, anxious), eXtraversion (confident, enjoy social gatherings, positive feelings), Agreeableness (peaceful, gentle, patient, agreeable), Conscientiousness (diligent, organized, planning), and Openness (curiosity, imaginativeness, depth). In what follows we provide a brief overview of work done to explore how personality is related to the ways in which a person interacts with the information environment.

Calvillo et al. ( 2021 ) reported that individuals higher on Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness, and lower on Extraversion, showed better accuracy in discriminating between real and fake news headlines (Calvillo et al., 2021 ). Hence, it is plausible that individuals high on these traits tend to authenticate whether headlines are true before sharing them online. Several published studies support this notion. Sampat and Raj ( 2022 ) found that individuals higher in Agreeableness and Conscientiousness had an increased tendency to vet news stories before sharing them. By contrast, they found that those higher in Extraversion and Neuroticism demonstrated an elevated tendency to share news stories online prematurely rather than vetting them. In addition, higher Agreeableness has been associated with a lower tendency to interact with suspicious Facebook posts (Buchanan and Benson, 2019 ). In sum, personality traits seem to shape how content in the information environment is regarded and treated.

While most of the existing research on personality and misinformation rely on the traditional Big Five model of personality (John et al., 1991 ), this model does not include the trait of Honesty-Humility—a trait that has been implicated in online behavior. For example, Honesty-Humility has been found to predict a person’s tendency and willingness to behave in a careless manner in the information environment as indicated by its strong association with the frequency of risky online behaviors, and feelings of disinhibition when operating online (D’Agata and Kwantes, 2020 ). Although there is a dearth of research studying Honesty-Humility and fake news belief, the related construct of Intellectual Humility (the tendency to be humble about, and acknowledge the limitations of, one’s own knowledge and understanding; Krumei-Mancuso and Rouse, 2016 ) provides theoretical support for the role of Honesty-Humility. Intellectual Humility is associated with less susceptibility to fake news and conspiratorial beliefs (Bowes and Tasimi, 2022 ) and more fact-checking behavior when encountering misinformation (Koetke et al., 2022 ; Koetke et al., 2023 ). In sum, Honesty-Humility seems to represent a highly predictive personality trait of the extent to which a person is susceptible to being influenced in the information environment. The first goal of this article, therefore, is to augment the contribution provided by Calvillo et al. ( 2021 ) that measured personality using the traditional five-factor model by including an examination of how Honesty-Humility contributes to the relationship using the HEXACO personality inventory (Ashton et al., 2014 ; Ashton and Lee, 2007 ).

An understanding of how personality traits influence susceptibility to online misinformation would be enhanced by an exploration of how the socially aversive dark triad traits of personality might also be related. Hodson et al. ( 2018 ) conducted a meta-analysis from which they concluded that the dark triad traits, which consist of psychopathy (i.e., impulsivity, lack of empathy), narcissism (i.e., a grandiose sense of self), and Machiavellianism (i.e., manipulative, amoral, and cunning) are not distinct from Honesty-Humility, but rather correspond to tendencies at the low end of the Honesty-Humility spectrum. Irrespective of whether high scores on the dark traits correspond to low Honesty-Humility, past research has identified an association between conspiratorial beliefs and the dark personality traits, however the pattern of results are inconsistent. For example, in a set of three studies, Cichocka et al. ( 2016 ) found that narcissism was related to conspiracy theory belief. Similarly, Lantian et al. ( 2017 ) found that a greater need for uniqueness, which is a feature of narcissism (Emmons, 1984 ), leads to greater belief in conspiracy theories. By contrast, March and Springer ( 2019 ) found that Machiavellianism and psychopathy, and not narcissism, predict conspiratorial beliefs. Relatedly, willingness to conspire has been reported to mediate the relationship between Machiavellianism and conspiratorial belief, such that that individuals who conspire against others tend to believe that they are likely to be conspired against (Douglas and Sutton, 2011 ). In more recent work, still others have found that all three dark triad traits were related to COVID-19 specific conspiracy theory belief (Giancola et al., 2023 ; Hughes and Machan, 2021 ). The differences across studies may be partially explained by the different scales used to measure the dark triad traits among studies. Overall, the dark triad traits may lead to conspiratorial belief due to one’s need to feel special and know things others do not (an element of narcissism), a high level of mistrust in others (present in psychopathy), or the high willingness to conspire (exhibited in high Machiavellianism).

What is less clear from our examination of the literature is the role that the dark personality traits play in the detection of misinformation in the information environment. Recent research conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic found that, similar to individuals who believed misinformation about COVID-19, individuals higher in the dark triad traits were less likely to partake in health promoting behaviors (Ścigała et al., 2021 ; Zajenkowski et al., 2020 ), like handwashing (Nowak et al., 2020 ; Triberti et al., 2021 ), mask-wearing (Chávez-Ventura et al., 2022 ), and willingness to get vaccinated (Howard, 2022 ) than people lower on these traits. However, this pattern may not necessarily be due to misinformation belief, it could indicate a general tendency for people high on the dark traits to behave in a selfish, self-centered, self-important manner. The most convincing study to suggest that the dark triad traits could be associated with misinformation detection was by Triberti et al. ( 2021 ) who reported that individuals higher on all three dark triad traits endorsed spreading alarming news before verifying it was true. However, it is unclear whether the pattern exhibited by Triberti et al.’s participants indicated an inability to discern misinformation, or the lack of motivation to do so. To better understand the role that personality plays in one’s identification of misinformation, we have included a measure of the Dark Triad traits alongside the HEXACO traits to determine how the collection of personality traits predicts one’s discernment of true from fake news headlines. Based on the results reported by Triberti et al., and the evidence suggesting that the dark triad traits represent component features associated with low Honesty-Humility (Hodson et al., 2018 ), we predict that greater narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism should be associated with a decreased ability to discern real from fake news stories.

Careless online behavior

Besides personality, the current study examines how one’s propensity to engage in unsafe or risky online behaviors and interactions might make them less sensitive to misinformation propagated within the information environment. Individuals who feel increased disinhibition online show an increased willingness to participate in risky online behaviors, such as disclosing personal information to others, and a higher frequency of falling victim to social engineering attacks (D’Agata and Kwantes, 2020 ). In addition, individuals who report a willingness to form relationships online have an increased tendency to disclose personal information, leaving them vulnerable to deception, whereas people who are more suspicious of others’ intentions online tend to be more conscientious, and thus may be better protected against deception from others online (D’Agata et al., 2021 ).

The way in which users interact with information and others online seems therefore to be shaped by the level of trust (or level of suspicion) they feel in exploring or consuming information from unknown sources and forming relationships with individuals who have only ever presented themselves online. It has been established that personality factors are associated with one’s tendency and willingness to engage in risky online behaviors (D’Agata and Kwantes, 2020 ) and one’s willingness to form relationships with others completely online (D’Agata et al., 2021 ). Common to both studies are the idea that some people have a high tolerance for risk when operating online—a risk that seems to have its basis in a tendency to trust the information presented to them. We postulate therefore, that the factors that put people at risk for social engineering and romance scams might also be at play in one’s tendency to believe unverified content presented in the information environment. Hence, in the current study, we tested the hypothesis that people who have a willingness, tendency, or history of reckless online behavior will exhibit a diminished ability to discern real from fake news headlines.

Demographic characteristics

In the misinformation and fake news literature, the effects of a variety of demographic variables have been studied, including gender, religiosity, and age. In the following section, we outline the current evidence for demographic differences in susceptibility to misinformation and fake news.

The effect of gender on belief in misinformation is inconsistent in the literature. Some studies have found that women had a higher prevalence of sharing misinformation on social media than men (e.g., Chen et al., 2015 ), while others have found no significant gender differences in detecting or sharing misinformation (Almenar et al., 2021 ; Mansoori et al., 2023 ). These differences are likely due in part to the heterogeneity between studies, particularly in the types of misinformation used and cultural differences in the sample. For example, Mansoori et al. ( 2023 ) noted that despite finding no gender effects, they had predicted an impact of gender because the sample used men and women from the United Arab Emirates, a traditionally patriarchal culture where genders differ in access to news and technology. Most of the research on gender differences in conspiracy theories have also found null results (e.g., Farhart et al., 2020 ; Miller et al., 2016 ), though some research using COVID-19 conspiracy theories specifically found women reported less belief than men (Cassese et al., 2020 ; Kim and Kim, 2021 ). It is likely the case that gender does not have a consistent effect on all fake news and misinformation belief and may play a more significant role when certain types of misinformation are used, such as when gendered narratives are leveraged to influence conspiracy theory belief (Bracewell, 2021 ). A discussion of gendered narratives is beyond the scope of this article, but a review of the literature makes clear that gender is an important demographic variable to measure and test in the current study.

Religious beliefs

Religiosity has also been implicated as a correlate to misinformation belief. Greater religious beliefs have been associated with belief in fake news (Bronstein et al., 2019 ), and political and medical conspiracy theories (Galliford and Furnham, 2017 ). This susceptibility to misinformation may be due to differences in thinking styles. Religious skeptics showed more use of an analytical thinking style (Pennycook et al., 2012 ; Pennycook et al., 2014 ; Shenhav et al., 2012 ), less errors on logical reasoning problems (Pennycook et al., 2013 ), and better performance on tests of critical thinking skills (Pennycook et al., 2016 ). Based on these associations in the literature, we expect to find that participants who self-report as high on a scale of religiosity will be less skilled at discerning fake from real news than those who report low religiosity.

Age is a point of focus in the misinformation literature; Many media literacy campaigns target young people (e.g., Schulten, 2022 ) and there is evidence that exposure to fake news and misinformation can have deleterious effects on youth (Dhiman, 2023 ). This specific focus on youth could be due to the significant amount of time younger generations spend on the internet and social media (Pérez-Escoda et al., 2021 )—a place where fake news and misinformation proliferate. Compounding this exposure risk is that younger people may not have developed the critical thinking skills (Kuhn, 1999 ) required to combat misinformation. Indeed, there is evidence that younger people tend to believe general conspiracy theories (Galliford and Furnham, 2017 ) and fake news (Halpern et al., 2019 ), more than older people. We expect to replicate the reported patterns in the current study.

The current research

In this study, we sought to examine the role that personality, the propensity for careless or risky online behavior, gender, religiosity, and age play in predicting one’s accuracy in discerning real from fake content presented in the online information environment. It is our view that with a better understanding of the antecedents of misinformation belief, educational materials to enhance media literacy can be enhanced to reduce the public’s susceptibility to it. However, with respect to personality, we acknowledge that traits are considered relatively fixed and resistant to change. Accordingly, our goals for the work presented here exclude any suggestion that resilience to misinformation requires adjustments to one’s personality. Instead, we hope that training materials for media literacy can incorporate our findings about personality to include components that explain how personality relates to susceptibility to misinformation belief, and perhaps even include personality measures to promote self-awareness about how one’s own traits could put them at risk online.

The literature suggests that personality influences one’s susceptibility to misinformation (e.g., Calvillo et al., 2021 )—a finding we predict will be replicated in the current study. The current research augments the work testing the relationships to the Big Five personality traits by Calvillo et al. ( 2021 ) to include a consideration of how Honesty-Humility (and the three dark triad personality traits that may be associated with those low on the Honesty-Humility dimension) relates to a person’s accuracy in discerning real from fake information. Based on work demonstrating a relationship between intellectual humility and belief in fake news and conspiracy theories (Bowes and Tasimi, 2022 ), an association between low Honesty-Humility and a heightened tendency to make oneself vulnerable while operating online (e.g., (D’Agata and Kwantes, 2020 ), and the evidence suggesting that the dark triad traits represent component features associated with low Honesty-Humility (Hodson et al., 2018 ), we predicted that ( H1 ) lower Honesty-Humility and ( H2 ) higher scores on the dark triad traits of narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism would be associated with decreased accuracy in discerning real from fake information.

We also predicted that those who show a willingness and tendency to interact with information and strangers online in manners that put them at risk for falling victim to scams or other online criminal acts (e.g., D’Agata et al., 2021 ) will also demonstrate a lack judgment when faced with new information of ambiguous veracity. Hence, we hypothesized that individuals who tend to be ( H3 ) disinhibited and reckless online (i.e., higher scores on the careless online behavior measures) will perform more poorly in a task that asks them to discern real from fake information than those who show good judgement online.

Finally, we collected demographic data to identify factors that relate to discernment of truthful from false information. We expected to demonstrate the same patterns with respect to age and religion already documented in the literature, such that ( H4 ) lower age and ( H5 ) less religiosity will be related to better accuracy in distinguishing between real and fake news. In terms of gender, the results in the literature are inconsistent, likely due in part to the heterogeneity of study methods, especially the types of misinformation used across studies. Since we do not employ specific gendered narratives and use a wide range of misinformation in our fake news content, ( H6 ) we did not expect to find gender differences in accuracy in distinguishing between real and fake news.

Participants

This research was granted approval by our organization’s Human Research Ethics Committee. We conducted an a priori power analysis based on a small effect size and determined a goal sample size of 500 participants. We used Qualtrics Panels to recruit participants, which is a web-based survey platform that allows researchers to build and monitor surveys, while Qualtrics Panels distributes the survey links and collects data on the researchers’ behalf. Footnote 2 To be eligible for the study, participants had to be between the ages of 18 and 80, live in the US or Canada, and be fluent in English. In addition, partial responses were not recorded, and those who failed any of the validity checks for random responses were removed from the sample. Further, respondents with completion times that were extreme outliers or those who took 50% less than the median time to complete the survey were removed.

The sample ( N  = 510) comprised of Canadian and American adults (see Table 1 ). The sample included 315 females and 188 males (four participants identified outside of the gender binary), ranging in age from 18–80 ( M  = 39.8, SD = 15.8). In our sample, 83.3% had some post-secondary education, and 60.6% of the sample was currently employed.

Demographic questionnaire

Participants completed questions on demographic characteristics, including gender, age, highest level of education, and current employment status. In addition, they completed a religious belief scale to assess religiosity and spirituality (Pennycook et al., 2016 ). The questionnaire contains items such as, “I believe in heaven” and “I believe in demons,” and uses a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) .

Online behavior measures

Risky online behaviors.

We used the Risky Online Behaviors scale (ROB; D’Agata and Kwantes, 2020 ) which includes a range of online behaviors for which participants are asked to indicate how often they have engaged in each. Items were rated using a 5-point frequency scale: 0 (never) , 1 (1 time) , 2 (2 times) , 3 (3–5 times) and 4 (more than 5 times) . The measure is not a psychometric tool but rather a count of the frequency with which a person has engaged in risky online behaviors. Behaviors range from the relatively benign (“ I’ve posted personal information in comments on social media that were public” ) to ones that make the participant vulnerable (e.g., “ I’ve disclosed a password to one of my own accounts via email” ). The measure indicated excellent internal consistency (Cronbach’s α  = 0.94).

Online Disinhibition

We used the Online Disinhibition scale (OD; D’Agata and Kwantes, 2020 ) to assess the extent to which individuals feel disinhibited online in the manner described by Suler ( 2004 ). The measure includes 20 items designed to measure comfort and feelings of anonymity while operating online. Example items include, “ I don’t need to monitor my behavior online as much as I do offline” and “ I have a different personality online than I do in the real world” . Items are rated using a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) . The measure indicated good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α  = 0.90).

Openness to Form Online Relationships

We used the Openness to Form Online Relationships scale (OFOR; D’Agata et al., 2021 ) to assess one’s willingness to develop friendships and romantic relationships with strangers online. The 6-items are rated on a 5-point rating scale, ranging from 1 ( strongly disagree ) to 5 ( strongly agree ) and consists of two subscales: Engagement, a measure of a person’s level of comfort in engaging with strangers online (good internal consistency, Cronbach’s α  = 0.84) and Suspicion, a measure of a person’s level of mistrust of others while operating online (acceptable internal consistency, Cronbach’s α  = 0.63).

Personality measures

Hexaco personality inventory.

We used the 100-item HEXACO Personality Inventory —Revised (HEXACO–PI–R; Lee and Ashton, 2018 ), to assess the six-factor model of personality (Honesty-Humility, Emotionality, eXtraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness). Participants rated the items (e.g., “people sometimes tell me that I am too critical of others” and “I avoid making ‘small talk’ with people” using a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) . The subscales indicated good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α ’s = 0.74–0.85).

The Short Dark Triad

We used the 27-Item Short Dark Triad (SD3; Jones and Paulhus, 2014 ) to measure the three socially aversive dark triad personality traits (narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy). Participants were asked to rate the items (e.g., “ It’s not wise to tell your secrets” , and “I like to get acquainted with important people” ) using a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) . The subscales indicated good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α ’s = 0.75–0.83).

Discerning real from fake information: The Headline Task

Twelve real and twelve fake headlines were selected from Peter et al.’s ( 2021 ) headline task which used real news and fake news headlines published on the internet and was adapted from Pennycook and Rand ( 2019 ). Mainstream news sources, such as CBC News and The New York Times were used to select real news headlines. These sources fell anywhere from far-left to far-right on the political spectrum. The real news headlines included topics such as climate change, government spending, and teen drug use. Headlines categorized as fake were selected from articles labeled as ‘false’ by Snopes.com, a fact-checking resource that directly evaluates information in the news. Examples of news stories featured in the fake news headlines include inmates outliving life sentences, micro-chipping campaigns, and animal limit laws. The headlines were categorized into three bins based on data from Peter et al. ( 2021 ) where headlines were classified as high accuracy (i.e., most of their participants accurately identified that the headline was either real or fake), 50–50 accuracy (i.e., ~50% of their participants were accurate), and low accuracy (i.e., most of their participants did not accurately identify that the headline was either real or fake). We selected eight headlines from each bin (four real and four fake) to use in the current study.

Participants were shown each headline separately and were told that while some may be real or accurate, others may not be. For each headline, participants were asked to rate whether they think the headline is from a real or fake news story. Task performance was assessed using measures borrowed from signal detection theory (Stanislaw and Todorov, 1999 ). Specifically, sensitivity, or accuracy in distinguishing between real and fake headlines was assessed by calculating the statistic d’ . Response bias (i.e., a tendency for one response to be favored over the other regardless of veracity) was assessed by calculating the statistic c . Footnote 3

Following the completion of the demographic questionnaire, participants completed the Headline Task where real and fake headlines were presented in randomized order. Next, they completed the Online Disinhibition Scale, Risky Online Behavior Scale, Openness to Form Online Relationships Scale, The HEXACO-PI-R, and the SD3 in a randomized order.

Descriptive statistics for all measures are reported in Table 2 and correlations among all measures are reported in Table 3 .

Headline task accuracy

First, we examined the associations between performance on the Headline Task and the demographic variables, using two-tailed Pearson’s correlation tests with an alpha level of 0.05. Age was not significantly correlated with headline task accuracy ( r  = 0.03, p  = 0.49). Religiosity was negatively correlated with headline task accuracy, such that people who were lower on religiosity were better at distinguishing between real and fake news headlines ( r  = −0.24, p  < 0.001). A two-tailed paired samples t -test with an alpha level of 0.05 indicated no significant difference in headline task performance between men and women ( t (501) = −0.77, p  = 0.44).

As expected, the different personality and online behavior variables were associated with one’s discernment of real from fake news. We conducted two-tailed Pearson’s correlation tests with a Bonferroni corrected alpha level of 0.004 to control for the Type I error rate due to multiple comparisons (see Table 3 for correlations among all measures). The correlational analysis provided support for our a priori predictions that poorer performance on the Headline Task would be associated with lower feelings of suspicion towards others’ intentions online ( r  = 0.16), greater willingness to engage in relationships with strangers online ( r  = −0.19), and greater online disinhibition ( r  = −0.23), and greater risky online behaviors ( r  = −0.18; all p ’s < 0.001). With respect to the personality measures, lower Honesty-Humility ( r  = 0.22), lower Openness ( r  = 0.16), and lower Conscientiousness ( r  = 0.20) were associated with poorer performance on the Headline task, as were greater Machiavellianism ( r  = −0.19), greater narcissism ( r  = −0.22), and greater psychopathy ( r  = −0.25; all p ’s < 0.001).

Headline task response bias

To test whether personality and individual differences were associated with a particular response pattern, we correlated response bias (i.e., c ) with the personality and online behavior measures (see Table 3 ). A positive c statistic indicated a tendency toward responding “true” to a headline regardless of its veracity, and a negative c statistic indicated a tendency toward responding ‘false’. The two-tailed Pearson’s correlation tests with a Bonferroni corrected alpha level of 0.004 indicated a significant bias toward rating the headlines as true among those who reported greater online disinhibition ( r  = 0.23), greater risky online behaviors ( r  = 0.28), and greater willingness to engage with strangers online ( r  = 0.19; all p ’s < 0.001). The same bias was found among those reporting higher levels of Machiavellianism ( r  = 0.22), narcissism ( r  = 0.21), and psychopathy ( r  = 0.26; all p’ s < 0.001). Finally, lower Honesty-Humility ( r  = −0.21, p  < 0.001) was associated with a bias toward indicating a headline is true. We did not find that suspicion of others’ intentions online, and the remaining personality traits, were significantly correlated with response bias.

Overall, our findings suggest that there are psychological and personality factors that affect the accurate assessment of the veracity of information encountered online. Individuals who self-reported as less diligent and organized (lower Conscientiousness), less curious and imaginative (lower Openness), and less sincere and honest (lower Honesty-Humility) demonstrated a diminished discernment between fake from real headlines. Consistent with the features of those who score low on Honesty-Humility, those high on the dark triad traits, and therefore those who self-report as more manipulative and cunning (higher Machiavellianism), more selfish and paranoid (higher Narcissism), and more impulsive and callous (higher Psychopathy) exhibited the same reduced accuracy, and a bias toward considering headlines as being ‘true’. Finally, those who were careless online (i.e., feeling more disinhibited online, more willing to engage with and share personal information with strangers online, or being less suspicious of strangers’ intentions online) were less accurate in discerning between fake and real news headlines, and similarly exhibited a bias towards considering the headlines to be true.

Our results confirm previously reported associations between personality and susceptibility to misinformation and provide some novel contributions. Although our personality results partially replicated Calvillo et al. ( 2021 ) with greater fake news detection accuracy being associated with higher Openness and Conscientiousness, unlike Calvillo et al., correlations for the role of Extraversion and Agreeableness did not reach significance; however, this could have resulted from the use of a different personality assessment tool or any differences between our task and theirs.

The novel contribution to the body of work is our finding that low levels of Honesty-Humility, and its associated high scores on dark triad traits, are related to a reduced accuracy in distinguishing real from false information, and an apparent bias toward interpreting information they encounter as true—a pattern suggesting that the lack of accuracy in identifying fake news from real news could be due to the tendency to consider all information as truthful. While results in the literature are mixed on the relationship between the dark triad traits and conspiratorial belief, with some studies finding only some of the triad traits having significant associations (e.g., Cichocka et al., 2016 ; March and Springer, 2019 ), our results align most closely with Hughes and Machan ( 2021 ) and Giancola et al. ( 2023 ) who found that greater levels of all three dark triad traits were associated with greater endorsement of conspiracy theories. Our results complement and build upon their findings by raising the possibility that the lower accuracy in distinguishing real from fake news headlines among those who score low on Honesty-Humility may be because the dark triad traits represent Honesty-Humility’s key features (n.b. Hodson et al., 2018 ). Based on our analysis of response bias, we propose further that individuals with greater dark triad traits could be especially susceptible to accepting conspiracy theories (Cichocka et al., 2016 ; Douglas and Sutton, 2011 ; Giancola et al., 2023 ; Hughes and Machan, 2021 ; Lantian et al., 2017 ; March and Springer, 2019 ) because of a general tendency or bias to believe information, thus making them more apt to consider far-fetched claims about government conspiracies to be believable or probable.

In addition to the personality factors we explored, our data showed that engaging in careless online behavior, including one’s willingness to form relationships with others online, was associated with a diminished accuracy in discerning real from fake content in the online environment, and with a bias toward considering information as true. In past research, greater feelings of disinhibition while operating in the online environment was associated with an increased frequency of risky online behaviors and an openness to forming online relationships, as well as less suspicion toward others’ intentions online (D’Agata and Kwantes, 2020 ; D’Agata et al., 2021 ). Our data replicate this pattern which, in addition to supporting their notions about the drivers of online misbehavior, helps to confirm the validity of their measures for OD and OFOR as predictors for performance on a task that measures detection of fake from real news headlines. A person’s increased willingness to put themselves at risk in the information environment through interactions with either information or strangers, or put another way, the increased trust they are willing to place in information or strangers in the information environment, raises the likelihood that they will be inaccurate when distinguishing between fake and real information, and that they will interpret information presented to them online at face value, rather than approaching it with skepticism.

Lastly, our findings are generally consistent with those of others who have explored the demographic factors that influence receptivity to misinformation. We found no evidence of gender differences in accuracy on our fake news task, which used headlines across a broad range of topics, and is in line with much of the literature on misinformation and gender (e.g., Almenar et al., 2021 ; Farhart et al., 2020 ; Mansoori et al., 2023 ; Miller et al., 2016 ). It is possible that gender differences emerge depending on the type of misinformation, such as the findings of greater belief in COVID-19 conspiracy theories by men than women (Cassese et al., 2020 ; Kim and Kim, 2021 ), or when gendered narratives are employed (Bracewell, 2021 ). Our results were also consistent with previous findings regarding age and religious beliefs. In line with Halpern et al. ( 2019 ) and Galliford and Furnham ( 2017 ) we found that younger participants were less able to identify fake information than older participants. We also found that greater religiosity was associated with lower accuracy when distinguishing between fake and real news, in line with other findings that have linked religious beliefs and the acceptance of misinformation as true (Bronstein et al., 2019 ; Galliford and Furnham, 2017 ). The replication of the age and religious effects is interesting. With respect to age, one could surmise that younger individuals’ lack of life experience may limit their willingness to question new information. For those who hold established institutions up as credible sources of information, individuals may be increasingly willing to consume new information as true without an accompanying inclination or drive to verify information on their own. In either case, the results point to a need for measures to promote critical thinking among consumers of information.

Implications

A common thread in our findings is the idea that aspects of trust seem to play a role in distinguishing truthful from false information. Individuals who tend to be less sincere and honest, more impulsive, manipulative, and self-involved – and therefore less trustworthy– are more likely to accept information as being true, perhaps due to an underlying belief that because they tend to be sneaky and deceitful, they are immune to being duped themselves. We also found that individuals who were more trusting of strangers online and thus engaged more frequently in relationships with them and shared more personal information online were also more likely to accept information as true. Together, the results point to ways in which training and education programs for media literacy could be designed as interventions for reducing susceptibility to misinformation and fake news. At a high level, our results point to the possibility that training for media literacy should highlight the importance of thinking critically about the information one encounters, as well as the value of approaching new information with the acknowledgement or understanding that any new information might be false, whether it is intentionally meant to deceive or not.

More importantly however, our results can inform efforts to incorporate the psychology of misinformation detection into educational materials designed to protect individuals operating online. As mentioned above, we acknowledge personality to be relatively fixed, thus efforts to change it is not the goal. Instead, we propose that media literacy materials which include personality measures and explanations about how personality traits relate to susceptibility to misinformation, as well as creating awareness of one’s own susceptibility to misinformation could be highly useful. For example, training that emphasizes the importance of diligence and the careful assessment of new information may resonate with those high on Conscientiousness, or high on Honesty-Humility—groups that are, based on our findings, at a lowered risk for belief in misinformation to begin with. But those who score low on Conscientiousness or Honesty-Humility could benefit from training materials that draw their attention to their own susceptibility and the personal costs of believing misinformation. The materials could also suggest new habits that could be developed to counter some of those tendencies. Training that incorporates this aspect of self-awareness about how one’s personality traits place them at risk for believing misinformation may be critical in encouraging people to resist initial tendencies to accept new information as true.

Education regarding the dangers of risky online behaviors could be leveraged as well. Media literacy programs that teach individuals to be suspicious of others’ intentions online and to evaluate information with a critical eye, and importantly, programs that include information about how trainees’ own individual tendencies could put them at risk when assessing information online could be of value. Equipped with some knowledge about how one’s own personality relates to how new information and interactions experienced online will be processed, individuals may be better able to calibrate themselves with respect to any biases they may possess with respect to their propensity to trust new information or online actors. A strategy used in some media literacy training is to encourage fact-checking new information using sources that are considered reliable. This not only encourages users of the information environment to approach new information with some degree of skepticism, but it is also consistent with best practices in Western intelligence communities who are trained to seek validation or falsification of information provided during intelligence collection. Intelligence analysts verify new information because the cost of being wrong is great. In a similar fashion, the consequences of having one’s behavior and attitudes shaped by unverified information posted online can also be significant when it is misleading or false.

In addition to our recommendations regarding the content of media literacy training, the finding that younger and more religious individuals showed a diminished accuracy in distinguishing real from fake news point to schools and religious institutions as potential routes of delivery for these training programs. The implementation of media literacy training programs in schools should continue to grow as part of the educational curriculum. In addition, community religious leaders could, as trusted messengers, play a role in countering misinformation, as well as in informing community members about the importance of facing new information with a critical perspective. Regardless of the intended audience for such programs, further work to develop, customize, and personalize media literacy programs for the digital age may be essential to combat the uptake of misinformation.

Limitations and future directions

We note that the effect sizes for our analyses were small. Small effect sizes are common in psychological research especially when studying a complex psychological phenomenon (Funder and Ozer, 2019 ; Götz et al., 2022 ) such as belief in, and detection of, fake news. Although our results indicated that personality traits and reckless online behavior online may play a role in predicting detection of misinformation, the variables we tested are not exhaustive. In our view, the tendency and ability to think critically, and the willingness to fact-check and scrutinize online information are also factors to explore for inclusion in media literacy programs designed as interventions to counter the threat of fake news belief.

As with many studies that use self-report measures, the data used in our study may be influenced by socially desirable responding, especially for our measure of the dark triad which asks about anti-social behaviors that many may be hesitant to admit committing, as well as the online behavior measures, which could be embarrassing to admit to engaging in. Past research also indicates a potential for inaccurate self-reporting of online behavioral tendencies (Parry et al., 2021 ). Future research could use alternative measures, such as peer reports of careless online behavior and fake news belief to verify the self-report measures. Future research could also survey individuals who have fallen for fake news in the past, or communities who deeply believe misinformation or conspiracy theories, to investigate their personality traits and behavioral tendencies online.

As a correlational study with somewhat limited scope with respect to the number of variables being studied, we cannot say for certain that there are no other, yet unexplored, intervening variables that also play a role in fake news detection. As such, the extent to which we can claim that careless online behaviors and personality traits are causing performance on the headline task has limits. A fuller treatment of the variables that underpin one’s difficulty in discerning real from fake news could be explored in future studies that implement longitudinal designs or experimental research. While expensive and time consuming to conduct, such studies may go some way to help us identify the causal nature of the effects. Lastly, future work to replicate the effects with pre-registration would be beneficial to enhance confidence in the reproducibility of our findings.

In sum, personality traits and careless online behavior appear to influence one’s discernment of real from fake information online. The findings were not altogether surprising given the results reported by D’Agata and Kwantes ( 2020 ) and D’Agata et al. ( 2021 ). However, they point to potentially useful directions for increasing the efficacy of media training by including a component that encourages self-awareness about one’s own potential to be influenced by information and strangers encountered online. Our results pertaining to the dark triad traits and the manner in which they explain how Honesty-Humility predicts enhanced detection of, and potentially one’s subsequent susceptibility to, misinformation is potentially the most theoretically interesting finding of this article because it exposed a common theme across our variables related to trust—among those who seem predisposed to trust misinformation and strangers online, are those who themselves should not be fully trusted. Indeed, the concept of trust could form the core of media literacy training models that concentrate on raising people’s self-awareness about their own susceptibility to online misinformation.

Data availability

The datasets generated during the current study are available in the Open Science Framework repository, https://osf.io/mscy6/?view_only=3cb2b1f8666045ab87ea5982cd91047c .

There are different types of false and inaccurate information that proliferates the internet. Specifically, disinformation refers to false or misleading information that is malicious and spread intentionally, whereas misinformation is false or misleading information but is spread without malice (Bennett and Livingston, 2018 ; Lewandowsky et al., 2013 ). Fake news is a more colloquial term, that refers to news articles or headlines that have been debunked or proven to be false. With the rise in internet use and political tensions, disinformation, fake news, and conspiracy theories are spreading at a rapid pace. Given the challenges associated with ascertaining if something was maliciously intended, we conceptualize fake news and conspiracy theories to be ‘misinformation’.

To ensure that those who participated in our earlier series of surveys did not participate in this study, all potential respondents were cross-checked by Qualtrics Panels.

Sensitivity is calculated as the z transform of the hit rate minus the z transform of the false alarm rate, and response bias is calculated as the z transform of the hit rate plus the z transform of the false alarm rate divided by −2. (Stanislaw and Todorov, 1999 ).

Allington D, Duffy B, Wessely S, Dhavan N, & Rubin J (2020) Health-protective behavior, social media usage and conspiracy belief during the COVID-19 public health emergency. Psycholog Med 51(10):1–7

Almenar E, Aran-Ramspott S, Suau J, Masip P (2021) Gender differences in tackling fake news: Different degrees of concern, but same problems. Media Commun 9(1):229–238

Article   Google Scholar  

Ashton MC, Lee K (2007) Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of the HEXACO model of personality structure. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 11(2):150–166

Ashton MC, Lee K, De Vries RE (2014) The HEXACO honesty-humility, agreeableness, and emotionality factors: a review of research and theory. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 18(2):139–152

Bennett WL, Livingston S (2018) The disinformation order: disruptive communication and the decline of democratic institutions. Eur J Commun 33(2):122–139

Bierwiaczonek K, Kunst JR, Pich O (2020) Belief in COVID‐19 conspiracy theories reduces social distancing over time. Appl Psychol Health Well‐Being 12(4):1270–1285

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Bowes SM, Tasimi A (2022) Clarifying the relations between intellectual humility and pseudoscience beliefs, conspiratorial ideation, and susceptibility to fake news. J Res Personal 98:104220

Bracewell L (2021) Gender, populism, and the QAnon conspiracy movement. Front Sociol 5:1–4

Bronstein MV, Pennycook G, Bear A, Rand DG, Cannon TD (2019) Belief in fake news is associated with delusionality, dogmatism, religious fundamentalism, and reduced analytic thinking. J Appl Res Mem Cogn 8(1):108–117

Buchanan T, Benson V (2019) Spreading disinformation on Facebook: do trust in message source, risk propensity, or personality affect the organic reach of “fake news”? Soc Media+ Soc 5(4):2056305119888654

Google Scholar  

Calvillo DP, Garcia RJ, Bertrand K, Mayers TA (2021) Personality factors and self-reported political news consumption predict susceptibility to political fake news. Personal Individ Differ 174:110666

Cassese E, Farhart C, Miller J (2020) Gender differences in COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs. Politics Gend 16(4):1009–1018. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1743923X20000409

Cichocka A, Marchlewska M, Golec de Zavala A (2016) Does self-love or self-hate predict conspiracy beliefs? Narcissism, self-esteem, and the endorsement of conspiracy theories. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 7(2):157–166

Chávez-Ventura G, Santa-Cruz-Espinoza H, Domínguez-Vergara J, Negreiros-Mora N (2022) Moral disengagement, dark triad and face mask wearing during the COVID-19 pandemic. Eur J Investig Health, Psychol Educ 12(9):1300–1310

PubMed   Google Scholar  

Chen X, Sin SCJ, Theng YL, Lee CS (2015) Why students share misinformation on social media: Motivation,gender, and study-level differences. J Acad Libr 41(5):583–592

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

D’Agata MT, Kwantes PJ (2020) Personality factors predicting disinhibited and risky online behaviors. J Individ Differ 41(4):199–206

D’Agata MT, Kwantes PJ, Holden RR (2021) Psychological factors related to self-disclosure and relationship formation in the online environment. Personal Relatsh 28(22):230–250

Dhiman B (2023) The rise and impact of misinformation and fake news on digital youth: a critical review. J Socialomics , 12(3)

Douglas KM (2021) COVID-19 conspiracy theories. Group Process Intergroup Relat 24(2):270–275

Douglas KM, Sutton RM (2011) Does it take one to know one? Endorsement of conspiracy theories is influenced by personal willingness to conspire. Br J Soc Psychol 50(3):544–552

Earnshaw VA, Eaton LA, Kalichman SC, Brousseau NM, Hill EC, Fox AB (2020) COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs, health behaviors, and policy support. Transl Behav Med 10(4):850–856

Emmons RA (1984) Factor analysis and construct validity of the narcissistic personality inventory. J Personal Assess 48(3):291–300

Farhart CE, Miller JM, Saunders KL (2020) Conspiracy stress or relief? Learned helplessness and conspiratorial thinking. In The politics of truth , eds. Elizabeth Suhay and David Barker, 1–38. Oxford University Press

Funder DC, Ozer DJ (2019) Evaluating effect size in psychological research: sense and nonsense. Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci 2(2):156–168

Galliford N, Furnham A (2017) Individual difference factors and beliefs in medical and political conspiracy theories. Scand J Psychol 58(5):422–428

Giancola M, Palmiero M, D’Amico S (2023) Dark triad and COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy: the role of conspiracy beliefs and risk perception. Curr Psychol , 1–13

Götz FM, Gosling SD, Rentfrow PJ (2022) Small effects: the indispensable foundation for a cumulative psychological science. Perspect Psychol Sci 17(1):205–215

Grebe E, Nattrass N (2012) AIDS conspiracy beliefs and unsafe sex in Cape Town. AIDS Behav 16(3):761–773

Halpern D, Valenzuela S, Katz J, Miranda JP (2019) From belief in conspiracy theories to trust in others: Which factors influence exposure, believing and sharing fake news. In: Meiselwitz G (eds) Social computing and social media. design, human behavior and analytics. HCII 2019. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol 11578. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-21902-4_16

Hodson G, Book A, Visser BA, Volk AA, Ashton MC, Lee K (2018) Is the dark triad common factor distinct from low honesty-humility? J Res Personal 73:123–129

Howard MC (2022) The good, the bad, and the neutral: vaccine hesitancy mediates the relations of Psychological Capital, the Dark Triad, and the Big Five with vaccination willingness and behaviors. Personal Individ Differ 190:111523

Hughes S, Machan L (2021) It’s a conspiracy: Covid-19 conspiracies link to psychopathy, machiavellianism, and collective narcissism. Personal Individ Differ 171:110559

John, OP, Donahue, EM, Kentle, RL (1991). Big five inventory. J Personal Soc Psychol

Jolley D, Douglas KM (2014) The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions. PLoS ONE 9(2):1–9

Jones DN, Paulhus DL (2014) Introducing the short dark triad (SD3) a brief measure of dark personality traits. Assessment 21(1):28–41

Kim S, Kim S (2021) Searching for general model of conspiracy theories and its implication for public health policy: analysis of the impacts of political, psychological, structural factors on conspiracy beliefs about the COVID-19 pandemic. Int J Environ Res Public Health 18(1):266

Koetke J, Schumann K, Porter T (2022) Intellectual humility predicts scrutiny of COVID-19 misinformation. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 13(1):277–284

Koetke J, Schumann K, Porter T, Smilo-Morgan I (2023) Fallibility salience increases intellectual humility: implications for people’s willingness to investigate political misinformation. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 49(5):806–820

Krumei-Mancuso EJ, Rouse SV (2016) The development and validation of the comprehensive intellectual humility scale. J Personal Assess 98(2):209–221

Kuhn D (1999) The developmental model of critical thinking. Educ Res 28(2):16–25

Lantian A, Muller D, Nurra C, Douglas KM (2017) “I know things they don’t know!”: The role of need for uniqueness in belief in conspiracy theories. Soc Psychol 48(3):160

Lee K, Ashton MC (2018) Psychometric properties of the HEXACO-100. Assessment 25:543–556

Lewandowsky S, Stritzke WGK, Freund AM, Oberauer K, Krueger JI (2013) Misinformation, disinformation, and violent conflict: from Iraq and the “war on terror” to future threats to peace. Am Psychol Assoc 68:487–501

Mansoori A, Tahat K, Tahat D, Habes M, Salloum SA, Mesbah H, Elareshi M (2023) Gender as a moderating variable in online misinformation acceptance during COVID-19. Heliyon, 9(9)

March E, Springer J (2019) Belief in conspiracy theories: the predictive role of schizotypy, machiavellianism, and primary psychopathy. PLoS ONE 14(12):e0225964. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225964

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Miller JM (2020) Psychological, political, and situational factors combine to boost COVID-19 conspiracy theory beliefs. Can J Political Sci/Rev Can Sci Politique 53(2):327–334

Miller JM, Saunders KL, Farhart CE (2016) Conspiracy endorsement as motivated reasoning: the moderating roles of political knowledge and trust. Am J Political Sci 60(4):824–844

Nowak B, Brzóska P, Piotrowski J, Sedikides C, Żemojtel-Piotrowska M, Jonason PK (2020) Adaptive and maladaptive behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic: the roles of Dark Triad traits, collective narcissism, and health beliefs. Personal Individ Differ 167:110232

Parry DA, Davidson BI, Sewall CJR, Fisher JT, Mieczkowski H, Quintana DS (2021) A systematic review and meta-analysis of discrepancies between logged and self-reported digital media use. Nat Hum Behav 5(11):1535–1547. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01117-5

Pennycook G, Cheyne JA, Barr N, Koehler DJ, Fugelsang JA (2014) Cognitive style and religiosity: the role of conflict detection. Mem Cogn 42(1):1–10

Pennycook G, Cheyne JA, Koehler DJ, Fugelsang JA (2013) Belief bias during reasoning among religious believers and skeptics. Psychonom Bull Rev 20(4):806–811

Pennycook G, Cheyne JA, Seli P, Koehler DJ, Fugelsang JA (2012) Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal belief. Cognition 123(3):335–346

Pennycook G, Rand DG (2019) Lazy, not biased: Susceptibility to partisan fake news is better explained by lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning. Cognition 188:39–50

Pennycook G, Ross RM, Koehler DJ, Fugelsang JA (2016) Atheists and agnostics are more reflective than religiousbelievers: Four empirical studies and a meta-analysis. PLoS One 11(4):e0153039

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Peter E, D’Agata M, Kwantes P, & Vallikanthan J (2021) Individual differences in susceptibility to disinformation: an example using the COVID-19 pandemic . Defence Research and Development Canada

Pérez-Escoda A, Pedrero-Esteban LM, Rubio-Romero J, Jiménez-Narros C (2021) Fake news reaching young people on social networks: Distrust challenging media literacy. Publications 9(2):24

Pummerer L, Böhm R, Lilleholt L, Winter K, Zettler I, Sassenberg K (2022) Conspiracy theories and their societal effects during the COVID-19 pandemic. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 13(1):49–59

Romer D, Jamieson KH (2020) Conspiracy theories as barriers to controlling the spread of COVID-19 in the US. Soc Sci Med , 263, 113356

Sampat B, Raj S (2022) Fake or real news? Understanding the gratifications and personality traits of individuals sharing fake news on social media platforms. Aslib J Inf Manag 74(5):840–876

Schulten K (2022) Teenagers and misinformation: some starting points for teaching media literacy . The New York Times . https://www.nytimes.com/2022/10/20/learning/lesson-plans/teenagers-and-misinformation-some-starting-points-for-teaching-media-literacy.html

Ścigała KA, Schild C, Moshagen M, Lilleholt L, Zettler I, Stückler A, Pfattheicher S (2021) Aversive personality and COVID-19: a first review and meta-analysis. Eur Psychol 26(4):348

Shenhav A, Rand DG, Greene JD (2012) Divine intuition: cognitive style influences belief in God. J Exp Psychol Gen 141(3):423–428

Stanislaw H, Todorov N (1999) Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 31(1):137–149

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Suler J (2004) The online disinhibition effect. Cyberpsychol Behav 7(3):321–326

Thorburn S, Bogart LM (2005) Conspiracy beliefs about birth control: Barriers to pregnancy prevention among African Americans of reproductive age. Health Educ Behav 32(4):474–487

Triberti S, Durosini I, Pravettoni G (2021) Social distancing is the right thing to do: Dark Triad behavioral correlates in the COVID-19 quarantine. Personal Individ Differ 170:110453

van Prooijen JW, Acker M (2015) The influence of control on belief in conspiracy theories: conceptual and applied extensions. Appl Cogn Psychol 29(5):753–761

Zajenkowski M, Jonason PK, Leniarska M, Kozakiewicz Z (2020) Who complies with the restrictions to reduce the spread of COVID-19? Personality and perceptions of the COVID-19 situation. Personal Individ Differ 166:110199

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Defence Research and Development Canada, Toronto, ON, Canada

Erika L. Peter, Peter J. Kwantes, Madeleine T. D’Agata & Janani Vallikanthan

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

Erika Peter: Investigation, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing—Original draft preparation, Writing—Review and editing, Writing—Approval of final version Peter Kwantes: Conceptualization, Writing—Original draft preparation, Writing—Review and editing, Writing—Approval of final version Madeleine D’Agata: Conceptualization, Writing—Original Draft, Writing—Review and editing, Writing—Approval of final version Janani Vallikanthan: Investigation, Data curation, Writing—Original Draft, Writing—Review and editing, Writing—Approval of final version. All authors have read and approved the submitted manuscript. All authors contributed to research conception and design; data acquisition, analysis, and interpretation; and manuscript preparation.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Erika L. Peter .

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval.

This research was performed in line with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. Approval was granted by the Human Research Ethics Committee of Defence Research and Development Canada—Toronto Research Centre (No. 2020-009).

Informed consent

All participants provided informed consent.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Peter, E.L., Kwantes, P.J., D’Agata, M.T. et al. The role of personality traits and online behavior in belief in fake news. Humanit Soc Sci Commun 11 , 1126 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-024-03573-6

Download citation

Received : 11 September 2023

Accepted : 12 August 2024

Published : 02 September 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-024-03573-6

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

research on personality reveals that it is

share this!

August 9, 2017

What social media reveals about your personality

by New England Complex Systems Institute

social media

Since the inception of social media, a prodigious amount of status updates, tweets, and comments have been posted online. The language people use to express themselves can provide clues about the kind of people they are, online and off. Current efforts to understand personality from writing samples rely on theories and survey data from the 1980s. New research from the New England Complex Systems Institute (NECSI) uses social media data to successfully identify differences and similarities between users without prior assumptions. This is a step toward building a better understanding of the psychology of human personality.

Some personality psychologists do study publicly available social media data in addition to solicited surveys. However, they still start with predefined traits like extroversion, neuroticism or narcissism and correlate them with the writing. In other research, linguists have used algorithms to identify topics of conversation, but they do not have much to say about the personalities of the conversationalists. NECSI's approach uses an unguided process that identifies word usage patterns among individual users without prior assumptions.

NECSI researchers analyzed tens of thousands of posts on the social media website Reddit to understand the characteristics of its users. The words written by each user were tallied and sorted, creating a distribution of individuals arranged according to the similarities or differences in their vocabularies. Pronouns and other very common words were not used to identify the array of users. Yet surprisingly, users still had strong variations in pronoun use. The way people refer to themselves and others has previously been correlated with personality. The new analysis also associates these personality markers with topics of conversation.

Topics emerged from the data as clusters of words frequently used by similar subsets of users. These word clusters include interests or hobbies like "hockey," "global politics," and "video games." Other topics have no obvious theme. Because these topics emerge from the data through an unguided process, they are not biased by prior assumptions.

Importantly, this analysis preserves the complex relationships between topics and pronoun use. For example, a cluster of users was identified with the topic "hockey." They frequently used words like NHL, puck, ice, Bruins, and Canucks. These users also typed a lot of third person male pronouns (he, his, him), reflecting the male-dominance of the sport. They also frequently used first person plural pronouns (we, us, our), suggesting a focus on teamwork.

"Understanding the ways people can be different from each other is one of the most exciting topics in science," NECSI president and an author of the paper, Prof. Yaneer Bar-Yam said. "This paper shows how we can make progress."

These preliminary findings establish the potential for identifying differences between individuals from abundant social media data. The words people use online can tell us about their patterns of behavior. This analysis can also lead to a better understanding of personality , informing existing psychological models.

Provided by New England Complex Systems Institute

Explore further

Feedback to editors

research on personality reveals that it is

Scientists use AI to unlock protein structures of hundreds of viruses for the first time

10 minutes ago

research on personality reveals that it is

Scientists uncover diverse marine microbes with potential for new antibiotics and plastic breakdown

research on personality reveals that it is

Uncollected waste and open burning major sources of plastic pollution, global study finds

research on personality reveals that it is

Major leap for nuclear clock paves way for ultraprecise timekeeping

research on personality reveals that it is

Experimental evolution study shows how microbial cooperation can emerge

27 minutes ago

research on personality reveals that it is

Staph's Achilles' heel may hinge on its choice of food

36 minutes ago

research on personality reveals that it is

Computer model boosts detection of cell-to-cell communication

49 minutes ago

research on personality reveals that it is

Quantum error correction research reveals fundamental insights into quantum systems

51 minutes ago

research on personality reveals that it is

New population model identifies phases of human dispersal across Europe

52 minutes ago

research on personality reveals that it is

Team identifies a 'forcefield-like' defense system in solid tumors and the genetic elements that can switch it off

58 minutes ago

Relevant PhysicsForums posts

Biographies, history, personal accounts.

2 hours ago

Why are ABBA so popular?

4 hours ago

Definition of Maoil

11 hours ago

Favorite songs (cont.)

Sep 2, 2024

Purpose of the Roman bronze dodecahedrons: are you convinced?

Talent worthy of wider recognition.

More from Art, Music, History, and Linguistics

Related Stories

research on personality reveals that it is

Facebook status updates reveal low self-esteem and narcissism

May 25, 2015

Data mining of Twitter posts can help identify when people become sympathetic to groups like ISIS

Jun 3, 2016

research on personality reveals that it is

Messages with moral-emotional words are more likely to go viral on social media

Jun 26, 2017

research on personality reveals that it is

Women use warmer language than men on Facebook, finds new research

May 26, 2016

research on personality reveals that it is

Analysis says social networking services may foster users' negative perceptions about their own popularity, happiness

Jun 21, 2017

Study twitter-maps new world order

Feb 19, 2012

Recommended for you

research on personality reveals that it is

Simulation study explores how gift giving drives social change

21 hours ago

research on personality reveals that it is

How new words arise in social media

research on personality reveals that it is

The right to be wrong: How context or human rationality may influence our decisions

research on personality reveals that it is

Early exposure to diverse faces helps babies overcome prejudices later in life, study suggests

research on personality reveals that it is

Warning labels from fact checkers work—even if you don't trust them—says study

Sep 3, 2024

research on personality reveals that it is

Study finds people are consistently and confidently wrong about those with opposing views

Aug 28, 2024

Let us know if there is a problem with our content

Use this form if you have come across a typo, inaccuracy or would like to send an edit request for the content on this page. For general inquiries, please use our contact form . For general feedback, use the public comments section below (please adhere to guidelines ).

Please select the most appropriate category to facilitate processing of your request

Thank you for taking time to provide your feedback to the editors.

Your feedback is important to us. However, we do not guarantee individual replies due to the high volume of messages.

E-mail the story

Your email address is used only to let the recipient know who sent the email. Neither your address nor the recipient's address will be used for any other purpose. The information you enter will appear in your e-mail message and is not retained by Phys.org in any form.

Newsletter sign up

Get weekly and/or daily updates delivered to your inbox. You can unsubscribe at any time and we'll never share your details to third parties.

More information Privacy policy

Donate and enjoy an ad-free experience

We keep our content available to everyone. Consider supporting Science X's mission by getting a premium account.

E-mail newsletter

research on personality reveals that it is

What’s Your True Generation? Truity’s Latest Test Reveals Your Generational Values

  • Latest Research
  • Personality & Culture

Were you born in the wrong generation? 

It’s no secret that different generations have stereotypes based on perceived characteristics and values. For example, we all know that Millennials are obsessed with avocado toast — and Baby Boomers are obsessed with complaining about how much Millennials love to eat avocado toast. 

But which generational tropes hold up? And how typical is it for an individual to resonate with their actual generation’s values? After digging into existing generational literature and theories developed by sociological researchers, we developed the True Generation test . This free test identifies your “true generation” based on how well your values and attitudes match with the idealized profile of each living generation: Gen Z, Millennial, Gen X, Baby Boomer and Silent. 

If you think it sounds a little extreme to categorize a group that’s comprised of millions of unique individuals, then you should know that is not the goal of the test. The True Generation test is not a personality test, and it’s also not meant to generalize every individual based on which twenty-year era they were born in. 

Instead, we were interested in how collective values are shaped by factors such as historical events, cultural trends and personal experiences. We looked at existing models like the Strauss–Howe generational theory and recent research examining generational differences. What we found were some patterns that offer a solid glimpse of each generation’s core values. 

From your working style (the “Puritan work ethic” belongs to the Boomers and Silents; Gen X, Millennials and Zoomers are all about that work/life balance) to how you vote (Gen Z and Millennials are increasingly liberal ), generational values do shape individuals and society. 

“Research shows that each generation has its own characteristic set of values, shaped by the historical, social and economic factors experienced during that generation’s formative years,” said Molly Owens, CEO and Founder of Truity. 

“After mapping these values from existing literature, we went a step further to outline how behaviors and attitudes could be linked with these generational values. We all know someone who just seems to be transported from a different era — the old soul, or the person born before their time. We thought it would be fun and illuminating to find a way to quantify how well you fit into the generation you were born in — as well as the generation where you really belong!”

Overview of Generations 

Before we look at each generation, a few disclaimers: 

  • Generational cut-off points aren’t a science and there is some debate on when each generation begins and ends, particularly for the Gen Z and Millennial generations. For our research, we defined the generational cohorts based on the dates provided by the Pew Research Center , which has conducted a significant amount of generational research. 
  • While there are some living members of the Greatest Generation (born before 1928), their modern societal influence is less prominent than the five generations we researched. We also excluded Generation Alpha, the generation that represents those under 12. 
  • And finally, you may be technically part of a generation and share very few of its values. That’s not uncommon at all (we’re looking at all the “old souls” and “young at hearts” out there). If that’s you, take the test to find out what generation you belong in. 

Gen Z (Born between 1997-2012) 

“Don’t try so hard to fit in, and certainly don’t try so hard to be different. Just be you.” — Zendaya  

It’s the authenticity for Gen Z. Gen Z is the first generation to be born into an entirely digital world. They’re more educated , tech-savvy and less happy than previous generations. According to the Pew Research Center, Gen Z is the most racially and ethnically diverse generation in the United States. Like Millennials, they tend to share pro-government and politically liberal values. 

Generational literature describes Gen Z as flexible, creative and independent. The Strauss–Howe model calls Gen Z an artist generation, as they were born and raised during a time of societal unrest and are channeling their energy into creative endeavors such as art, music and storytelling on platforms like TikTok. From a historical standpoint, Gen Z’s values are influenced by events such as the COVID-19 pandemic, the climate crisis, gun violence in schools, and the rise in social media and artificial intelligence. 

Values-driven Zoomers strive to live by their principles and expect the same from the brands they consume and the organizations for which they work. While still young, Gen Z is already leading movements on climate change ( climate activist Greta Thunberg launched a global campaign by boycotting school) and gun reform ( see the movement launched by Parkland high schoolers ). 

Millennials (Born between 1981-1996)

“I'm intimidated by the fear of being average.” — Taylor Swift

For the past thirty years, the conversation around Millennials was mainly focused on the generation as one of coddled children, entitled thanks to participation trophies and helicopter parents . But now, Millennials are all grown up — and they’re America’s largest generation at over 72 million people. Millennials were the first generation to grow up with the internet. They’re tech-savvy, collaborative and socially conscious. They’re also less likely to embrace traditional social expectations — Millennials are less likely to be married , are more likely to live with their parents as adults, and are waiting longer to have children than previous generations. 

Most generational literature describes Millennials as optimistic, open-minded, achievement-oriented and collaborative. Millennials value teamwork and civic engagement while championing causes like healthy work-life balance and diversity. They are unique in their collectivistic values, leading mass movements centered around minority or lesser-heard voices like Black Lives Matter, #metoo, immigration reform and LGBTQ+ rights. 

Strauss and Howe call Millennials the hero generation, as many Millennials are stepping into positions of power and leadership and feel responsible for fixing the issues society faces today. Millennial values are shaped by historical events such as the 9/11 terrorist attacks, the war in Iraq, the Great Recession and the rise of the internet and social media. 

Gen X (Born between 1965 and 1980)

“Wanting to be someone else is a waste of the person you are.” — Kurt Cobain

Because of its smaller size and influence compared to other generations, Gen X is sometimes called the “forgotten middle child” of the generations. But don’t let the stereotype fool you — Gen X makes up one-fifth of the population and has significantly contributed to technology and societal change. 

Generational literature describes Gen Xers as individualistic, informal, innovative and skeptical. Strauss and Howe call Gen X the nomad generation, as they were often under-protected and neglected as children (i.e., “latch-key” children of the 1970 and 80s ). Because of this, Gen Xers grew up with an individualistic nature and distrust of large groups and institutions. While coming of age, Gen Xers experienced such historical events as the Cold War, the AIDS epidemic, space exploration, the development of the personal computer and rise of the internet. 

Criticized for being slackers , Gen X refused to subscribe to the workaholic mentality of previous generations. They led the way in normalizing unconventional career paths, with many of this generation embracing entrepreneurship. Unlike the civic-minded generations on either side of them, Gen X is less politically engaged but made strides in the workplace fighting for work/life balance, diversity and technological advancement. 

Baby Boomers (Born between 1946 and 1964) 

“We live in a world where man has walked on the moon. It’s not a miracle; we just decided to go.” — Tom Hanks 

The Baby Boomer generation is known for being big — both in terms of its literal size and its societal impact. Boomers were the first generation in the U.S. to grow up with television , have disposable income and to expect that they would have a better life than their parents. As children and young adults, Baby Boomers experienced major historical events such as the Vietnam War, the Women’s Liberation Movement, the Civil Rights Movement and the Sexual Revolution. 

Generational literature describes Baby Boomers as optimistic, competitive and curious. This generation values a strong work ethic, teamwork and personal responsibility. The Strauss–Howe model calls Baby Boomers a prophet generation because they were born after a crisis (World War II) and are known for their idealism and desire to make a difference in the world.

Their optimism led Baby Boomers to participate in many social and political movements. The generation’s activism is responsible for some of the most significant social changes of the 20th century, including the passage of the Equal Rights Amendment, the legalization of abortion and the end of the Vietnam War. Baby Boomers questioned and rethought traditional methods in business and politics, modernizing existing fields and constructing new industries like tech and the wellness industry. 

Silent Generation (Born between 1928 and 1945)

“The best way to guarantee a loss is to quit.” — Morgan Freeman

Children of the Great Depression and World War II, their “Silent” label refers to their image as a conforming and civic-minded generation. Living through war and hardship, the Silent Generation, also known as Traditionalists, came to value patriotism, loyalty and a strong work ethic. An overall smaller generation, scholars believe the low birth rate was due to the uncertainty and challenging conditions of the period. 

Generational literature describes this generation as dependable, straightforward, loyal and obedient.  Some historical events that significantly impacted the Silent Generation are the Great Depression, McCarthyism and the Red Scare, and World War II. 

Strauss and Howe describe this generation as coming of age “too late to be war heroes and just too early to be youthful free spirits.” They entered adulthood as risk-averse professionals and homemakers who understood that following the status quo was their ticket to success. The generation is known for being frugal and saving rather than spending, which sets them apart from their Baby Boomer children. 

Why Do Generational Values Matter?

Today it feels as if we are more fractured along generational lines than ever before, but by understanding generational values, we may be more able to understand current societal issues, let go of the judgments and biases we hold for the generations that come before and after ours, and have a bit more empathy for those who see the world in different ways. 

What’s your true generation? Take the free test here . And if you're interested to learn more about why we're all so obsessed with generational differences, check out this blog post. 

Megan Malone Megan holds an MS in organizational psychology and manages content and brand marketing at Truity. She is passionate about helping people improve their relationships, careers, and quality of life using personality psychology. An INFJ and Enneagram 9, Megan lives quietly in Fort Worth, Texas with her husband and two pups. You can chat with her on Twitter @meganmmalone.

Who Are The Conspiracy Theorists? Their Motivations and Personality Traits, Revealed

  • Load More Articles

Courtesy Michael Varnum, Ph.D.

Cross-Cultural Psychology

3 things your name might reveal about you, humans and computers can match faces to names with surprising success..

Updated June 11, 2024 | Reviewed by Vanessa Lancaster

  • Research shows that people hold stereotypes about others based on their names.
  • Names can reveal cultural values and cultural shifts, research suggests.
  • Our names may also influence our personality.
  • Names may also influence important life decisions although this claim is more controversial.

What’s your name? It’s a common enough question we will likely answer hundreds or perhaps thousands of times during our lives. Beyond making social interactions less awkward and giving us a helpful tag to remember information about others, names can also reveal more than you might imagine about human psychology.

Names as Stereotypes

What type of person comes to mind when you think of someone named Elizabeth? How about someone named Misty? According to 400 American participants, “Elizabeths” are high in warmth and competence, while “Mistys” are low on both of these positive traits. “Rileys" are seen as warm but not competent, and “Ruths” are viewed as competent but not warm (Newman et al., 2018; 2022).

Other work suggests that we use names to infer information, often stereotypical, about others’ ethnicity and social class. In a series of experiments involving thousands of participants, Crabtree and colleagues (2022) showed that participants tended to guess that targets with names that are more common among European Americans, like “Mary,” are higher in both education and income than targets with names like “Lakisha” that are more common among African Americans.

Names more common among Latin Americans were linked with the lowest perceived income and education levels on average. In contrast, names more common among Asian Americans were linked to the highest perceived education levels and income levels comparable to those with names common among European Americans.

Names and Culture

Names can tell us something about culture, and have been used in a number of studies to trace geographic variations in values and cultural changes over time. Several researchers have argued that the choice of a relatively unpopular vs. popular name may indicate a preference for uniqueness or conformity —essentially, a way of helping your child to fit in or stand out.

And indeed, in parts of the world where individualism is higher, so is the proportion of children receiving relatively uncommon names (Ogihara, 2023; Varnum & Kitayama, 2011; 2022). Regional differences in naming practices also suggest that preferences for nonconformity are stronger in parts of the U.S. that were more recently the frontier, potentially reflecting historical and contemporary self-selection in migration to these regions (Varnum & Kitayama, 2011). Consistent with this idea, an analysis of Scandinavian census and migration records suggests that people with less-common names were more likely to emigrate historically (Knudsen, 2019).

Shifts in naming also may signal changes in cultural values over time. The proportion of babies receiving popular names has in fact declined dramatically over the past two centuries in the U.S. (Twenge et al., 2010; Grossmann & Varnum, 2015) and over the past few decades in Japan (Ogihara & Ito, 2022). Shifts which are consistent with other evidence of rising individualism in these societies over time (Hamamura, 2012; Santos, Varnum, & Grossmann, 2017).

Beyond reflecting a preference for conformity or non-conformity, names may be linked to what’s known as honor culture, a set of norms and values common in the American South emphasizing the importance of reputation and defense of reputation through aggression (Nisbett, 2018). An analysis of popular names by Brown and colleagues (2014) suggests patronymic names (giving a child their father’s first name) are more common in states where honor culture is more predominant. In a separate study, the researchers found that men who more strongly endorsed honor values reported a greater preference for giving any future children they might father their own first name.

Our Names, Our Destinies?

Intriguingly, recent studies suggest that we may look like our names. When asked to guess which of a handful of names corresponded to a photograph of a person’s face, humans and computers were able to do so at rates above chance (Zwebner et al., 2017), a phenomenon the researchers suggest may be due to a self-fulfilling prophecy.

The achieving peers

Our names may also influence where we end up living and what kind of careers we pursue. In a series of studies, Pelham and colleagues (2002) found that women named Virginia, as opposed to Mildred, were more likely to live in Virginia Beach. “Mildreds,” on the other hand, were more likely than “Virginias” to live in Milwaukee. The researchers also found that “Dennises” were disproportionately likely to be dentists. Results like these suggest that our names may shape these consequential life decisions through what the researchers call “implicit egotism.”

These findings are quite well-known—however, a set of replications and re-analyses by Simonsohn (2011) has cast doubt on whether in fact names are driving these effects. For example, although "Dennises" are more likely than "Walters" to be dentists, they are also more likely than "Walters" to be lawyers. Findings that Simonsohn argues suggest cohort effects in name frequencies rather than implicit egotism.

In later work, Pelham and Mauricio (2014) did find more robust support for the notion that our last names may influence career choices, i.e. "Carpenters" are more likely to be, you guessed it, carpenters. And a study published last year in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology using arguably more rigorous controls than in the original work does in fact find support for the idea that our names may influence important life choices (Promothesh, et al., 2023).

And, intriguingly, a series of recent studies by Bao and colleagues (2023) suggests links between the uniqueness of people's names and their creativity . People with unique names were viewed as better suited to creative occupations. Intriguingly the researchers found that films by Chinese directors with unique names were rated more highly than those made by their more conventionally named counterparts.

So do our names shape our destinies? The evidence increasingly suggests the answer may be yes.

Names aren’t a typical variable in psychological science, and most popular books on names have little science to them. Yet the study of names can provide valuable insights into phenomena ranging from cultural change to stereotypes. Our names may also be cues that shape not only others’ perceptions of us but also our personality and (perhaps) our choices about where to live and what to do for a living.

Facebook /LinkedIn image: GaudiLab/Shutterstock

Bao, H.-W.-S., Lu, H., & Luo, Y. L. L. (2023). Do unique names fit people for creative work? Implications for job recruitment, name change, and product evaluation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 53, 1524–1541

Brown, R. P., Carvallo, M., & Imura, M. (2014). Naming patterns reveal cultural values: Patronyms, matronyms, and the US culture of honor. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40 (2), 250-262.

Crabtree, C., Gaddis, S. M., Holbein, J. B., & Larsen, E. N. (2022). Racially distinctive names signal both race/ethnicity and social class. Sociological Science , 9 , 454-472.

Hamamura, T. (2012). Are cultures becoming individualistic? A cross-temporal comparison of individualism–collectivism in the United States and Japan. Personality and social psychology Review , 16 (1), 3-24.

Knudsen, A. S. B. (2019). Those who stayed: Individualism, self-selection and cultural change during the age of mass migration. SSRN. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3321790

Newman, L. S., Tan, M., Caldwell, T. L., Duff, K. J., & Winer, E. S. (2018). Name norms: A guide to casting your next experiment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin , 44 (10), 1435-1448.

Newman, L. S., Tan, M., Caldwell, T. L., Duff, K. J., & Winer, E. S. (2022). " Name norms: A guide to casting your next experiment": Corrigendum. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 48 (2), 328.

Nisbett, R. E. (2018). Culture of honor: The psychology of violence in the South . Routledge.

Ogihara, Y. (2023). Popular names are given less frequently to babies in individualistic countries: Further validation of unique names as an indicator of individualism. Current Research in Behavioral Sciences , 4 , 100094.

Ogihara, Y., & Ito, A. (2022). Unique names increased in Japan over 40 years: baby names published in municipality newsletters show a rise in individualism, 1979-2018. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology , 3 , 100046.

Pelham, B. W., Mirenberg, M. C., & Jones, J. T. (2002). Why Susie sells seashells by the seashore: implicit egotism and major life decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 82 (4), 469-487.

Pelham, B., & Mauricio, C. (2015). When Tex and Tess Carpenter build houses in Texas: Moderators of implicit egotism. Self and Identity, 14 (6), 692-723.

Chatterjee, P., Mishra, H., & Mishra, A. (2023). Does the first letter of one’s name affect life decisions? A natural language processing examination of nominative determinism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 125(5), 943–968.

Santos, H. C., Varnum, M. E., & Grossmann, I. (2017). Global increases in individualism. Psychological Science , 28 (9), 1228-1239.

Simonsohn, U. (2011). Spurious? Name similarity effects (implicit egotism) in marriage, job, and moving decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101 (1), 1–24.

Twenge, J. M., Abebe, E. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2010). Fitting in or standing Out: Trends in American parents' choices for children’s names, 1880–2007. Social Psychological and Personality Science , 1 (1), 19-25.

Varnum, M. E., & Kitayama, S. (2011). What’s in a name? Popular names are less common on frontiers. Psychological Science , 22 (2), 176-183.

Varnum, M. E. W., & Kitayama, S. (2022). "What’s in a name? Popular names are less common on frontiers": Corrigendum. Psychological Science, 33 (6), 1020-1021.

Zwebner, Y., Sellier, A. L., Rosenfeld, N., Goldenberg, J., & Mayo, R. (2017). We look like our names: The manifestation of name stereotypes in facial appearance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 112 (4), 527-554.

Courtesy Michael Varnum, Ph.D.

Michael E. W. Varnum, Ph.D. , is an associate professor and social psychology area head in the Department of Psychology at Arizona State University.

  • Find a Therapist
  • Find a Treatment Center
  • Find a Psychiatrist
  • Find a Support Group
  • Find Online Therapy
  • United States
  • Brooklyn, NY
  • Chicago, IL
  • Houston, TX
  • Los Angeles, CA
  • New York, NY
  • Portland, OR
  • San Diego, CA
  • San Francisco, CA
  • Seattle, WA
  • Washington, DC
  • Asperger's
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Chronic Pain
  • Eating Disorders
  • Passive Aggression
  • Personality
  • Goal Setting
  • Positive Psychology
  • Stopping Smoking
  • Low Sexual Desire
  • Relationships
  • Child Development
  • Self Tests NEW
  • Therapy Center
  • Diagnosis Dictionary
  • Types of Therapy

September 2024 magazine cover

It’s increasingly common for someone to be diagnosed with a condition such as ADHD or autism as an adult. A diagnosis often brings relief, but it can also come with as many questions as answers.

  • Emotional Intelligence
  • Gaslighting
  • Affective Forecasting
  • Neuroscience

AI Personality Reveal

This is the first test of its kind to infer your personality from written text using AI. Unlike other tests, the Personality Reveal uses advanced text analytics AI that doesn't rely on predefined answers.

Heading Background Image

How this test works

It's all about your writing.

Many personality tests use multiple choice questions and can therefore hardly grasp your true feelings and emotions. In this test, however, you'll get to answer three open-ended questions with no boundaries. This test is not meant to be taken just once. You can repeat it every day as the questions as well as your mood change.

Text Analytics AI

This AI personality test uses a unique psycholinguistic text analytics AI that has been trained to detect sentiment, emotions and personality traits from any written text. Being applied in large commercial projects to create consumer segments from social data, it has only recently been made available to the public .

Recommendation System

When you take the test, not only will you learn something you didn't know about yourself, it also supports you in developing and improving your personality. Carefully hand-picked articles, videos and podcasts that are tailored to your personality characteristics support you on your very own journey.

Why should I take the AI test?

The more you know yourself, but the more you're able to assert what you really need. Becoming aware of how you express your own thoughts and feelings through written text can make a huge impact on your life. Take this AI personality test to understand your personality traits better and learn how to embrace them.

Instagram profile of mackenziebustin

"I liked it a lot! My favorite part was that I could answer completely from my own thought and not pick from a list of things."

Instagram profile of barbara.y_

"In the beginning I was a bit sceptical on how it would work, though at the end I was surprised by its accuracy."

Instagram profile of my.sea.r

"It gave me really great results! Maybe too exact results!"

Instagram profile of mindful_psycho

"Test out one of the newest and coolest personality tests!"

The research

This AI personality test utilizes a combination of deep learning models provided through Symanto's psycholinguistic text analytics API to derive the personality, attitude towards life, mindset and emotions expressed through a person's writing.

Personality Traits Analysis

Personality traits are (relatively) enduring patterns of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors that distinguish individuals from one another (Roberts & Mroczek, 2008). Based on tonality and linguistic style of writing, this model can reflect the innate way that individuals make decisions.

Sentiment Analysis

Detecting the tonality of a written text in combination with a personality trait allows us to draw conclusions to the person's attitude to life and mindset. Being trained on datasets in native language, this model is said to grasp the language nuances of every author.

Emotion Analysis

While the sentiment model returns simplified positive and negative markers, the emotion model deeply analyses a full range of human emotions. It is trained on the universal emotions as defined by Dr. Paul Ekman: enjoyment, sadness, fear, anger, surprise and disgust.

Felix Melchner

Get in Touch

I'm available on the following social media platforms:

This website is currently in beta. Please follow the link to leave your feedback: https://forms.gle/wzkgZK5cwy7SCbn6A

Journal of Language Teaching and Research

The Effects of Problem-Based Learning on the Writing Skills of Students Across Various Personality Types

  • Jumariati Jumariati Universitas Lambung Mangkurat
  • Elvina Arapah Universitas Lambung Mangkurat

Problem-based learning (PBL) is an approach where group discussions and collaboration are apparent during problem-solving activities. Accordingly, learners’ personality types that affect the way they think, feel, behave, and interact may potentially have a role in PBL classrooms. This study tries to reveal the possible roles personality types play in PBL by investigating the effects of PBL on the argumentative essay writing of both extroverted and introverted students. This study employed a quasi-experimental design by randomly selecting students in academic writing courses for both the experimental and comparison groups and involving them in the intact classes. The findings revealed that the students in the PBL group scored higher than those in the guided writing group. Moreover, the extroverted students in the PBL group achieved higher mean scores than the extroverted students in the comparison group; however, the difference was insignificant. On the contrary, the statistical analysis showed that the introverted students in the experimental group outperformed those in the comparison group. This is to say that the introverted students taught using PBL had better skills in writing argumentative essays compared to those taught using guided writing techniques. This finding implies the need to use suitable teaching strategies that facilitate both extroverts and introverts in developing their writing skills while also sharpening their communicative and social skills.

Author Biographies

Jumariati jumariati, universitas lambung mangkurat.

English Language Education Study Program

Elvina Arapah, Universitas Lambung Mangkurat

Alavinia, P. & Hassanlou, A. (2014). On the Viable Linkages between Extroversion/Introversion and Academic Iranian EFL Learners’ Writing Proficiency. English Language Teaching, 7(3), 167-185. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/elt.v7n3p167

Boroujeni, A., Roohani, A. & Hasanimanesh, A. (2015). The Impact of Extroversion and Introversion Personality Types on EFL Learners’ Writing Ability. Theory and Practice in Language Studies, 5(1), 212-218. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.17507/tpls.0501.29

Burch, K. (2000). A Primer on Problem-Based Learning for International Relations Courses. International Studies Perspectives, 1(1), 31-44. Retrieved January 13th 2024 from http://www.jstor.org/stable/44218105 .

Cain, S. (2012). Quiet: The Power of Introverts in a World That Can't Stop Talking. New York, NY: Crown Publishers.

Cao, C. & Meng, Q. (2020). Exploring personality traits as predictors of English achievement and global competence among Chinese university students: English learning motivation as the moderator. Learning and Individual Differences, 77, Article 101814. DOI: https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/j.lindif.2019.101814

Dewaele, J. (2013). Personality in Second Language Acquisition. In The Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistic. Ed. Carol A. Chapelle. Blackwell Publishing Limited. DOI: 10.1002/9781405198431.wbeal0904

Dewaele, J. & Furnham, A. (1999). Extraversion: The Unloved Variable in Applied Linguistic Research. Language Learning, 49(3), 509–544. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/0023-8333.00098

Dörnyei, Z. & Skehan, P. (2003). Individual Differences in Second Language. In Catherine J. Doughty, Michael H. Long (eds.). The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition, 589-630. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470756492.ch18

Dörnyei, Z. (2006). Individual differences in second language acquisition. AILA Review, 19, 42–68. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/aila.19.05dor

Dow, S.E. (2013). The Invisible Students in the Classroom: How to Include the Introverts Without Excluding the Extroverts. Education and Human Development Master's Theses, 268. Retrieved January 13th 2024 from http://digitalcommons.brockport.edu/ehd_theses/268

Ellis, R. (1994). The Study of Second Language Acquisition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Graham, S. & Sandmel, K. (2011). The Process Writing Approach: A Meta-analysis. The Journal of Educational Research, 104(6), 396-407. DOI: http://doi.org./10.1080/00220671.2010.488703

Hajimohammadi, R. & Mukundan, J. (2011). Impact of Self-Correction on Extrovert and Introvert Students in EFL Writing Progress. English Language Teaching, 4(2), 161-168. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/elt.v4n2p161

Hallinger, P. & Lu, J. (2011). Implementing problem-based learning in higher education in Asia: challenges, strategies and effect. Journal of Higher Education Policy and Management, 33(3), 267-285, DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1360080X.2011.565000

He, T. (2019). Personality Facets, Writing Strategy Use, and Writing Performance of College Students Learning English as A Foreign Language. SAGE Open, 1-15. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244019861483

Hemmatnezhad, S., Jahandar, S. & Khodabandehlou, M. (2014). The Impact of Extraversion vs. Introversion on Iranian EFL Learners' Writing Ability. Indian Journal of Fundamental and Applied Life Sciences, 4(1), 119-128. Retrieved February 5, 2023 from http://www.cibtech.org/jls.htm

Ho, D. W. L., Whitehill, T. L. & Ciocca, V. (2014). Performance of speech-language pathology students in problem-based learning tutorials and in clinical practice. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 28(1–2), 83–97. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2013.812146

Hung, W. (2013). Problem-Based Learning: A Learning Environment for Enhancing Learning Transfer. New Directions for Adult and Continuing Education, 137, 27-38. (Online), ( http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ace.20042)

Johnson, S. M. & Finucane, P. M. (2000). The emergence of problem-based learning in medical education. Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice, 6(3), 281–291. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2753.2000.00267.x

Johnston, I. (2000). Essays and Arguments: A Handbook on Writing Argumentative and Interpretive Essays. Retrieved January 28, 2023 from http://www.mala.bc.ca/-johnston/arguments .

Jumariati, J. & Sulistyo, G.H. (2017). Problem-Based Writing Instruction: Its Effect on Students’ Skills in Argumentative Writing. Arab World English Journal (AWEJ), 8(2), 87-100. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.24093/awej/vol8no2.6

Khodabandeh, F. (2022). Exploring the applicability of virtual reality‐enhanced education on extrovert and introvert EFL learners’ paragraph writing. Educational Journal of Technology in Higher Education, 19-27. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s41239-022-00334-w

Kok, F. Z. & Duman, B. (2023). The effect of problem-based learning on problem-solving skills in English language teaching. Journal of Pedagogical Research, 7(1), 154-173. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.33902/JPR.202318642

Kumari, V., Ffytche, D. H. Williams, S. C. R. & Gray, J. A. (2004). Personality Predicts Brain Responses to Cognitive Demands. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24(47), 10636–10641. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3206-04.2004

Layeghi, F. (2011). Form and Content in the Argumentative Writing of Extroverted and Introverted Iranian EFL Learners. Iranian EFL Journal, 7(3), 166-183. Retrieved January 28, 2023 from http://www.iranian-efl-journal.com/2011

Li, X., & Liu, J. (2021). Mapping the taxonomy of critical thinking ability in EFL. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 41, 100880. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2021.100880

Li, Y. (2013). Effects of Problem-Based English Writing Instruction on Thai Upper Secondary School Students’ Critical Thinking Abilities and Argumentative Writing Skills. Online Journal of Education, 8(1), 242-255. Retrieved January 28, 2023 from http://www.edu.chula.ac.th./ojed

Liang, H. Y. & Kelsen, B. (2018). Influence of Personality and Motivation on Oral Presentation Performance. Journal of Psycholinguist Research, 47(2). DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-017-9551-6

Lieberman, M. D. & Rosenthal, R. (2001). Why Introverts Can't Always Tell Who Likes Them: Multitasking and Nonverbal Decoding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(2), 294-310. DOI: https://doi.org/10.I037//O022-3514.80.2.294

Lin, Y. (2018). Core Issues in Developing Critical Thinking Skills. In: Developing Critical Thinking in EFL Classes. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-7784-5_1

Liyanage, I. & Bartlett, B. (2013). Personality types and languages learning strategies: Chameleons changing colours. System, 41(3), 598-608. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2013.07.011

Marefat, F. (2006). Student Writing, Personality Type of the Student and the Rater: Any Interrelationship? The Reading Matrix, 6(2), 116-124. Retrieved February 2, 2023 from http://www.readingmatrix.com/articles/marefat/article.pdf

Nejad, A. M., Bijami, M. & Ahmadi, M. R. (2012). Do Personality Traits Predict Academic Writing Ability? An EFL Case Study. English Linguistics Research, 1(2). DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5430/elr.v1n2p145

Othman, N & Shah, M. I. A. (2013). Problem-Based Learning in the English Language Classroom. English Language Teaching, 6(3), 125-134. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/elt.v6n3p125

Oxford, R. (2003). Language learning styles and strategies: An overview. GALA. 1-25. Retrieved December 13th 2023 from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/254446824_Language_learning_styles_and_strategies_An_overview

Qanwal, S. & Ghani, M. (2019). Relationship Between Introversion/Extroversion Personality Trait and Proficiency in ESL Writing Skills. International Journal of English Linguistics, 9(4), 107-118. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5539/ijel.v9n4p107

Sanjaya, D., Mokhtar, A. A., & Sumarsih. (2015). The Impact of Personality (Extroversion/Introversion) on Indonesian EFL Learners’ Essay Writing Achievement. The Asian EFL Journal Professional Teaching Article, 87, 4-19. Retrieved November 12th 2023 from http://www.asian-efl-journal.com

Savery, J. R. (2006). Overview of Problem-Based Learning: Definitions and Distinctions. Interdisciplinary Journal of Problem-Based Learning, 1, 9-20. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7771/1541-5015.1002

Sharp, A. (2008). Personality and Second Language Learning. Asian Social Science, 11(4), 17-25. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/ass.v4n11p17

Shorkpour, N. & Moslehi, S. (2015). The relationship between personality types and the type of correction in EFL writing skill. Pertanika Journal of Social Science and Humanities, 23(1), 35-46.

Smalley, R. L., Ruetten, M. K. & Kozyrev, J. R. (2001). Refining Composition Skills: Academic Writing and Grammar (Developing & Refining Composition Skills). Heinle Cengage Learning.

Soland, J., Hamilton, L. S. & Stecher, B. M. (2013). Measuring 21st-century competencies: Guidance for educators. Asia Society: RAND Corporation.

Swanberg, A. B., & Martinsen, O. L. (2010). Personality, approaches to learning and achievement. Educational Psychology, 30(1), 75-88. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1080/01443410903410474

Yuan, R., Yang, M., & Lee, I. (2021). Preparing pre-service language teachers to teach critical thinking: Can overseas field school experience make a difference? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 40, 100832. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2021.100832

Zaswita, H. & Ihsan. R. (2020). The Impact of Personality Types on Students’ Writing Ability. Jurnal Pendidikan Indonesia, 9(1), 75-84. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.23887/jpi-undiksha.v9i1.21101

Copyright © 2015-2024 ACADEMY PUBLICATION — All Rights Reserved

More information about the publishing system, Platform and Workflow by OJS/PKP.

IMAGES

  1. Personality: Theory and Research, Fourteenth Edition

    research on personality reveals that it is

  2. Personality Theory & Research: Personality Theory & Research

    research on personality reveals that it is

  3. Myers Briggs Personality Test: Let’s Explore Your Personality Type!

    research on personality reveals that it is

  4. The 5 Personality Traits That Can Reveal Your Political Ideology: New

    research on personality reveals that it is

  5. What is Personality

    research on personality reveals that it is

  6. Exploring the Many Psychology Theories of Personality

    research on personality reveals that it is

VIDEO

  1. "Discover Your True Self: The Ultimate Personality Test"

  2. Probability Comparison: How Rare Is Your Personality Type

  3. How Impactful is Your Personality?

  4. Personality development aspects by Col.Rajeev Bharwan

  5. What your face tells about you

  6. Uncovering the hidden influence of birth order on personality

COMMENTS

  1. LifeSpan Chapter 19 Quiz Flashcards

    Research on personality reveals that it is: genetically influenced environmentally influenced not fixed, but instead a lifelong process. According to Erikson's theory, the crisis that follows identity achievement is called: intimacy versus isolation. Friends are crucial during emerging adulthood because they:

  2. Chapter 14 Flashcards

    Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like Research on day-to-day interactions by Gable, Reis, Downey finds, A personality style involving anxious expectations of rejection is called, Ayduck's research demonstrated that people could control their emotions regarding interpersonal relationships by focusing on and more.

  3. This Huge Study Is The Best Evidence Yet That Your Personality Changes

    This Huge Study Is The Best Evidence Yet That Your Personality Changes Over Time. You might be fundamentally you for your entire life, but don't expect your personality to stay the same. That's according to a major study of 50,000 people over the course of several decades, which found the traditional notion of personality - as fixed and ...

  4. When is personality revealed?: A motivated cognition approach

    What defines who someone really is? When is personality revealed? Many of us have an intuitive sense that some behaviors and situations unmask aspects of our underlying personality more than others. How high we sing, how far we jump, or how fast we walk are typically not accepted as evidence of our personality. On the other hand, individuals' ability to resist eating a tempting marshmallow ...

  5. New Research Identifies Personality Types That Are Naturally ...

    Extraverts have the upper hand when it comes to caring for their basic psychological needs. getty. A new study published in the Journal of Research in Personality identifies the types of people ...

  6. What Your Choice of Words Says about Your Personality

    What Your Choice of Words Says about Your Personality. A language analysis program reveals personality, mental health and intent by counting and categorizing words. By Jan Dönges. July 2009 Issue ...

  7. Personality: What is it and how does it reveal itself?

    We often hear the word 'personality' in our conversations. Usually it is used to describe a person. Sometimes the descriptions involving the word are favourable (e.g., "She has a warm personality") and sometimes they are unfavourable (e.g., "He has an aggressive personality"). There are psychological tests which attempt to.

  8. Research in Personality

    Interactionism and Personality. D. Magnusson, in International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2001 The target of scientific analysis in personality research is an active and purposeful organism, functioning and developing as a total, integrated being in interaction with different aspects of the environment.

  9. Personality Assessments: 10 Best Inventories, Tests, & Methods

    The scale has a solid theoretical and empirical grounding, making it a popular workplace assessment around the world. 4. NEO-PI-R. The NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 2008) is a highly popular self-report personality assessment based on Allport and Odbert's (1936) trait theory of personality.

  10. Who believes in conspiracies? New research offers a theory

    New research by Josh Hart, associate professor of psychology, suggests that people with certain personality traits and cognitive styles are more likely to believe in conspiracy theories. The ...

  11. The role of personality traits and online behavior in belief in fake

    The current study examines how careless online behavior and personality traits are related to the detection of fake news. We tested the relationships among accurately distinguishing between fake ...

  12. What social media reveals about your personality

    Facebook status updates reveal low self-esteem and narcissism. 19 shares. Feedback to editors. Since the inception of social media, a prodigious amount of status updates, tweets, and comments have ...

  13. What's Your True Generation? Truity's Latest Test Reveals Your

    For our research, we defined the generational cohorts based on the dates provided by the Pew Research Center, which has conducted a significant amount of generational research. While there are some living members of the Greatest Generation (born before 1928), their modern societal influence is less prominent than the five generations we researched.

  14. Chapter 2 Flashcards

    Cross-cultural research that investigates national character with the Five Factor Model of personality reveals that _____. a. people accurately judge others based on national character b. people's beliefs about national character are usually inaccurate stereotypes c. Canadians are more agreeable than people think d. there is a high correlation between stereotypes and data from real individuals

  15. The Idea of the Mobile Personality Revisited

    The brief descriptions above paint two rather different pictures of a "mobile personality"—one suggesting high levels of Conscientiousness (e.g., sense of duty, persistence) and ...

  16. Psychologists Reveal Personality Trait That Leads to Power

    FOLLOW. Pandora Dewan is a Senior Science Reporter at Newsweek based in London, UK. Her focus is reporting on science, health and technology. Pandora joined Newsweek in 2022 and previously worked ...

  17. 3 Things Your Name Might Reveal About You

    Here's what research reveals about our networks' gravitational force. Subscribe. Recent. Issue Archive. Today. News. ... Personality and social psychology Review, 16(1), 3-24.

  18. personality psych ch 1 & 2 quiz Flashcards

    Roots of personality psychology can be traced to the theater because. A. early studies utilized theater-goers as subjects (easily available) . B. actors have always been considered celebrities. C. actors often portray "characters" or easily recognizable types of people. D. Shakespeare was Freud's best (most insightful) disciple.

  19. Personality Reveal

    The more you know yourself, but the more you're able to assert what you really need. Becoming aware of how you express your own thoughts and feelings through written text can make a huge impact on your life. Take this AI personality test to understand your personality traits better and learn how to embrace them. Start the Test. @mackenziebustin.

  20. Search

    This link is provided for convenience only and is not an endorsement of either the linked-to entity or any product or service.

  21. Personality Chapter 3 Flashcards

    Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like There is evidence of individual differences in how people perceive and process information about the social world. Some psychologists refer to this as _____________ intelligence. a. perceptual b. emotional c. interpersonal d. social, The textbook defines __________ as the ability of individuals to "stand outside their immediate ...

  22. The Effects of Problem-Based Learning on the Writing Skills of Students

    Problem-based learning (PBL) is an approach where group discussions and collaboration are apparent during problem-solving activities. Accordingly, learners' personality types that affect the way they think, feel, behave, and interact may potentially have a role in PBL classrooms. This study tries to reveal the possible roles personality types play in PBL by investigating the effects of PBL ...

  23. Chapter 11 Flashcards

    a) personality types. Research on psychological traits reveals. a) strong cross-situational consistency in traits such as honesty. b) clear evidence that traits physically exist in the brain. c) none of the answers is correct. d) a lack of cross-situational consistency in traits such as honesty.