U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Campbell Syst Rev
  • v.19(1); 2023 Mar

Logo of csysrev

Communication skills training for improving the communicative abilities of student social workers

Emma reith‐hall.

1 Health and Life Sciences, De Montfort University, Leicester UK

2 Department of Social Policy and Social Work, University of Birmingham, Birmingham UK

Paul Montgomery

Associated data.

Good communication is central to effective social work practice, helping to develop constructive working relationships and improve the outcomes of people in receipt of social work services. There is strong consensus that the teaching and learning of communication skills for social work students is an essential component of social work qualifying courses. However, the variation in communication skills training and its components is significant. There is a sizeable body of evidence relating to communication skills training therefore a review of the findings helps to clarify what we know about this important topic in social work education. We conducted this systematic review to determine whether communication skills training for social work students works and which types of communication skills training, if any, were more effective and lead to the most positive outcomes.

This systematic review aimed to critically evaluate all studies which have investigated the effectiveness of communication skills training programmes for social work students. The research question which the review posed is: ‘What is the effectiveness of communication skills training for improving the communicative abilities of social work students?’ It was intended that the review would provide a robust evaluation of communication skills training for social work students and help explain variations in practice to support educators and policy‐makers to make evidence‐based decisions in social work education, practice and policy.

Search Methods

We conducted a search for published and unpublished studies using a comprehensive search strategy that included multiple electronic databases, research registers, grey literature sources, and reference lists of prior reviews and relevant studies.

Selection Criteria

Study selection was based on the following characteristics: Participants were social work students on generic (as opposed to client specific) qualifying courses; Interventions included any form of communication skills training; eligible studies were required to have an appropriate comparator such as no intervention or an alternative intervention; and outcomes included changes in knowledge, attitudes, skills and behaviours. Study selection was not restricted by geography, language, publication date or publication type.

Data Collection and Analysis

The search strategy was developed using the terms featuring in existing knowledge and practice reviews and in consultation with social work researchers, academics and the review advisory panel, to ensure that a broad range of terminology was included. One reviewer conducted the database searches, removing duplicates and irrelevant records, after which each record was screened by title and abstract by both reviewers to ensure robustness. Any studies deemed to be potentially eligible were retrieved in full text and screened by both reviewers.

Main Results

Fifteen studies met the inclusion criteria. Overall, findings indicate that communication skills training including empathy can be learnt, and that the systematic training of social work students results in some identifiable improvements in their communication skills. However, the evidence is dated, methodological rigour is weak, risk of bias is moderate to high/serious or incomplete, and extreme heterogeneity exists between the primary studies and the interventions they evaluated. As a result, data from the included studies were incomplete, inconsistent, and lacked validity, limiting the findings of this review, whilst identifying that further research is required.

Authors’ Conclusions

This review aimed to examine effects of communication skills training on a range of outcomes in social work education. With the exception of skill acquisition, there was insufficient evidence available to offer firm conclusions on other outcomes. For social work educators, our understanding of how communication skills and empathy are taught and learnt remain limited, due to a lack of empirical research and comprehensive discussion. Despite the limitations and variations in educational culture, the findings are still useful, and suggest that communication skills training is likely to be beneficial. One important implication for practice appears to be that the teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education should provide opportunities for students to practice skills in a simulated (or real) environment. For researchers, it is clear that further rigorous research is required. This should include using validated research measures, using research designs which include appropriate counterfactuals, alongside more careful and consistent reporting. The development of the theoretical underpinnings of the interventions used for the teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education is another area that researchers should address.

1. PLAIN LANGUAGE SUMMARY

1.1. communication skills training helps improve how social work students interact with the people they safeguard and support.

Communication skills training, including empathy training, can help social work students to develop their communication skills. Opportunities to practise communication skills in a safe and supportive environment through role‐play and/or simulation, with feedback and reflection, helps students to improve their skills. The effect of doing this training face‐to‐face, online or via blended learning is largely unknown.

1.2. What is this review about?

Good communication skills are important for social work practice and are commonly taught on social work qualifying courses. There is a range of different types of educational interventions, with wide variations in theoretical basis, approach, duration and mode of delivery. This systematic review looks at whether different interventions are effective in producing the following outcomes: social work students’ knowledge, attitudes, skills and behaviours.

What is the aim of this review?

This Campbell systematic review assesses whether communication skills training for social work students works, and which types of communication skills training, if any, were more effective and led to the most positive outcomes.

1.3. What studies are included?

This review summarises quantitative data from randomised and non‐randomised studies. The 15 studies included in this review were undertaken in Canada, Australia and North America. The research is very limited in terms of scope and quality, and there are important weaknesses in the evidence base.

1.4. Does communication skills training improve the communicative abilities of social work students?

Systematic communication skills training shows some promising effects in the development of social work students’ communicative abilities, especially in terms of their ability to demonstrate empathy and interviewing skills.

1.5. What do the findings of this review mean?

Communication is very important for social work practice, so we need to ensure that student social workers have opportunities to develop their communication skills.

Too few studies fully assessed student characteristics such as age, sex and ethnicity or took account of how previous experience, commitments and motivation affected students’ learning.

Consideration of stakeholder involvement and collaboration (such as by people with lived experience) was also lacking. Only the role of the educator was considered.

The studies were largely of poor quality and investigated many different implementation features, which made it difficult to draw any firm conclusions about what makes the teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education effective.

Researchers conducting studies into communication skills training should seek to carry out robust and rigorous outcomes‐focused studies. They should also consider trying to see how and where these interventions might work, as well as understanding for whom they may be effective.

1.6. How up‐to‐date is this review?

The review authors searched for studies that had been published until 15 June 2021.

2. BACKGROUND

2.1. description of the condition.

Good communication is central to social work practice (Koprowska,  2020 ; Lishman,  2009 ), underpinning the success of a wide range of social work activities. People in receipt of social work services value social workers who are warm, empathic, respectful, good at listening and demonstrate understanding and compassion (Beresford et al.,  2008 ; Department of Health,  2002 ; Ingram,  2013 ; Kam,  2020 ; Munford & Sanders,  2015 ; Social Care Institute for Excellence,  2000 ; Tanner,  2019 ). Even in diverse and challenging circumstances, effective communication is thought to build constructive working relationships and enhance social work outcomes (Healy,  2018 ).

Communication, sometimes referred to as interpersonal communication, ‘involves two (or more) people interacting to exchange information and views’ (Beesley et al.,  2018 ). It is driven and directed by the desire to achieve particular goals and is underpinned by perceptual, cognitive, affective and behavioural operations (Hargie,  2017 ). In social work practice and education, the values of the profession and the specific social, cultural, political and ideological contexts in which social workers operate, influence the nature of interpersonal communication (Harms,  2015 ; Koprowska,  2020 ; Thompson,  2003 ).

Research has tended to focus on particular aspects of communication, or the impact of communication in specific contexts. For example, a study examining how social workers communicate with parents in child protection scenarios, identified that social workers who demonstrated empathy towards simulated clients, encountered less resistance and more disclosure (Forrester et al.,  2008 ). Social workers who use creative and play‐based approaches confidently facilitate engagement and communication with children (Ferguson,  2016 ; Handley & Doyle,  2014 ). Adapting skills and strategies to address specific communication difficulties is equally important in social work with adults. Offering choices through pictographs can help people with Aphasia to answer open questions (Rowland & McDonald,  2009 ), whilst Talking Mats can facilitate conversation with people who have dementia (Murphy et al.,  2007 ). Research into the experiences and preferences of palliative care patients found small impactful supererogatory acts demonstrated compassion which allowed them to ‘feel heard, understood, and validated’ (Sinclair et al.,  2017 , p. 446). Communicating effectively with adults in receipt of health and social care services also enables them to better participate in important decisions about their care.

The impact of failing to communicate effectively has been well documented, particularly through reports into incidents of child deaths (Laming,  2003 ,  2009 ; Munro,  2011 ). Consequently, the importance of teaching communication skills to social work students as a means of enabling them to communicate effectively has long been recognised (Smith,  2002 ). More recently, there have been calls for the expansion and/or improvement of this training (Luckock et al.,  2006 ; Narey,  2014 ). Considerable time, effort and money has been spent on achieving this aim, leading to a wide range of communication skills training courses becoming embedded in social work programmes across the globe. Communication generally, and some communication skills specifically, feature in the educational standards of different countries including the Australian Social Work Education and Accreditation standards, the Educational Policy Accreditation Standards in the US and the Professional Capabilities Framework in the UK (Australian Association of Social Workers,  2020 ; British Association of Social Workers,  2018 ; Council on Social Work Education,  2015 ). One of the consequences of the coronavirus pandemic was increasing diversification of the delivery of teaching and learning in Higher Education. However, the impact of online or blended learning on the development of student social workers’ communication skills remains to be seen.

2.2. Description of the intervention

Communication skills training (CST) can be defined as ‘any form of structured didactic, e‐learning, and experiential (e.g., using simulation and role‐play) training used to develop communicative abilities’ (Papageorgiou et al.,  2017 , p. 6). Although ‘communication skills training’ (CST) is the name given to the intervention on a wide range of professional and vocational courses, in social work education, the intervention is more commonly referred to as the ‘teaching and learning of communication skills’; a trend reflected in the titles of various knowledge and practice reviews. Given purpose, role and context have a significant impact on communication in social work practice, conceptualisations which integrate knowledge, values and skills, for example the knowing, being and doing domains developed by Lefevre and colleagues (Lefevre et al.,  2008 ) have become increasingly popular (Ayling,  2012 ; Woodcock Ross,  2016 ). In social work education, the intervention includes not only communication processes, but also an understanding of the broader contextual issues in which those interactions in social work practice occur. This views communication in social work as both an art and a science (Healy,  2018 ), which alongside a move away from purely instructional methods, helps explain the preference for the term ‘teaching’ or ‘education’ rather than ‘training’ among social work academics and researchers. There is a tendency within this discipline for significant variation in terminology due to the wide knowledge base from which social work draws. The term ‘communication skills’ is not applied uniformly in the social work literature‐microskills, interpersonal skills and interviewing skills are frequently used alternatives.

In spite of a lack of consensus about what the intervention is called, ‘the inclusion of a dedicated communication skills module early in the course, or a strong communication component within an early module about methods, skills and practice’ is commonplace (Dinham,  2006 , p. 841). A consensus appears to be emerging in the wider social work literature, regarding what the basic communication skills for social work practice actually entail. These microskills comprise non‐verbal communication such as making eye contact and nodding, alongside a range of verbal techniques including clarifying, reflecting, paraphrasing, summarising and asking open questions. Described in detail in a number of social work text books (Beesley et al.,  2018 ; Cournoyer,  2016 ; Healy,  2018 ; Sidell & Smiley,  2008 ), and featuring in the educational standards, competency and capability frameworks of various countries (Australian Association of Social Workers,  2020 ; British Association of Social Workers,  2018 ; Council on Social Work Education,  2015 ), these skills form part of the content of a number of communication skills courses and preparation for practice modules. Microskills help social workers and social work students to ‘establish and maintain empathy, communicate non‐verbally and verbally in effective ways, establish the context and purpose of the work, open an interview, actively listen, establish the story or the nature of the problem, ask questions, intervene and respond appropriately’ (Harms,  2015 , p. 22). Microskills are considered to be transferable across client groups and settings.

Using case study scenarios that students might encounter in practice, microskills are rehearsed in different social work contexts or circumstances. Typically, students practice the microskills through undertaking simulated social work tasks such as assessments and care planning. When applied to social work tasks and contexts, communication skills are sometimes referred to within the social work literature as interviewing skills. An interview is a ‘person‐to‐person interaction that has a definite and deliberate purpose’ (Kadushin & Kadushin,  2013 , p. 6). It is through social work interviews that ‘important connections and relationships are developed, and where important concepts such as partnership and empowerment are taken forward’ (Trevithick,  2012 , p. 185).

The pedagogic practices used to teach communication skills to social work students include a wide range of affective, cognitive and behavioural components, whereby students participate in a variety of activities. Following face‐to‐face taught input including theory, communication skills are generally rehearsed, using role‐play with peers (e.g., Koprowska,  2003 ), simulated practice with service users (e.g., Moss et al.,  2007 ) or actors (e.g., Petracchi & Collins,  2006 ). Tutors and peers may also model communication skills to demonstrate different techniques. Critical reflection, which facilitates students’ self‐awareness is encouraged. Feedback is an important component in helping learners develop an understanding of their strengths and areas for development, and a range of feedback mechanisms are welcomed by students (Tompsett, Henderson, Mathew Byrne, et al.,  2017 ). Video and playback are often used to support the learning that occurs through feedback and reflective processes. Some universities have purpose‐built recording suites or provide students with equipment such as tablets to facilitate the recording of communication skills practice. The rationale for video and playback is ‘that each student's adult ability to be their own best assessor’ is ‘utilised to the full’ (Moss et al.,  2007 , p. 715); the value of which has been recognised by students elsewhere (Bolger,  2014 ; Cartney,  2006 ). A learning environment, characterised by trust, safety and security, appears to be an important mechanism for students to make use of experiential activities. Opportunities for observing skills in practice, through shadowing a social worker or allied practitioner, feature in some communication skills or preparation for practice modules. Attention may also be devoted to specific areas of communication: communicating with children, communicating with people who have hearing impairments, and inter‐professional communication are some examples.

No specific blueprint for CST in social work exists, thus the nature of the training sessions and course length vary from one educational institution to another. Typically, in the UK, CST is delivered to first year undergraduate and postgraduate students before they commence their first practice placement: in England, this may comprise some of the 30 days of skills training which universities typically provide. Content and teaching activities tend to be designed and delivered on an individual basis by social work academics, often with involvement from people with lived experience (service users and carers), practitioners and local employers. Examples of gap‐mending strategies for user involvement are beginning to find their way into the literature (Askheim et al.,  2017 ) and have been applied to the teaching and learning of communication skills (Reith‐Hall,  2020 ), however such activities are far from mainstream. Minimum requirements, dosage, and delivery methods are not prescribed, leading to considerable heterogeneity of the intervention in practice.

2.3. How the intervention might work

Training or education‐based interventions aimed at improving the communicative abilities of student social workers seek to bring about changes in learners’ knowledge, values and skills in terms of how to communicate effectively in social work practice.

Psychological perspectives and counselling theories, including the work of humanistic and client‐centred theorists such as Rogers, Carkhuff and Egan tend to underpin microskills training. Other communication theories, including the model of interpersonal communication developed by Hargie (Hargie,  2006 ) also provide a theoretical basis for the skills taught on some of these courses. Concerns have been raised that psychological and counselling theories have been applied to social work uncritically (Trevithick et al.,  2004 ), without due consideration of the challenges this may present. A number of social work academics have pulled together theory and research on communication skills in recent years (e.g., Beesley et al.,  2018 ; Harms,  2015 ; Healy,  2018 ; Koprowska,  2020 ; Lishman,  2009 ; Woodcock Ross,  2016 ) in an attempt to address this issue. Nonetheless, it still remains ‘difficult to identify a coherent theoretical framework that informs the learning and teaching of communication skills in social work’ (Trevithick et al.,  2004 , p. 18).

The theoretical underpinnings of the pedagogic practices used to teach communication skills are not always clear (Dinham,  2006 ; Trevithick et al.,  2004 ). The conception of reflection in and on action (Schön,  1983 ) and the importance of ‘learning by doing’ (Schön,  1987 , p. 17) are often cited as underpinning the teaching of communication skills modules in social work education. Experiential learning, ‘the process whereby knowledge is created through the transformation of experience’ (Kolb,  1984 , p. 38) is another of the prevailing philosophies, although Trevithick et al.,  2004 , p. 24) suggest there is an uncritical assumption that ‘experiential is best’. Reference is sometimes made to theories of adult learning, whereby students are expected to draw on their own experiences, take responsibility for their own learning, and engage in peer learning. This mode of learning ‘is understood to encourage the sustained internalisation of skills’ (Dinham,  2006 , p. 847). Such ideas build on the concept of andragogy (Knowles,  1972 ,  1998 ) whereby mutual processes of learning and growth are encouraged.

The knowledge review conducted by Trevithick et al. ( 2004 ) identified articles where the theoretical foundations for teaching skills in social work were made explicit. The communication skills module at the University of York in the UK, based on Agazarian's theory, is located within a systems framework (Koprowska,  2003 ). Relational teaching based on relational/cultural theory should underpin teaching in social work education, whereby mutual engagement, mutual empathy, and mutual empowerment foster growth in relationships between tutors and students (Edwards & Richards,  2002 ). These examples are the exception to the rule; few articles theorise the teaching and learning process (Eraut,  1994 ). Generally speaking, ‘communication skills have been taught, but not reflected upon; experienced, but not theorised’ (Moss et al.,  2007 , p. 711).

A wide variety of approaches for teaching communication skills to social work students exist in practice. Given there is more expertise in the teaching and learning of communication skills than the literature denotes, academics should continue theorising and researching this aspect of the curriculum (Dinham,  2006 ). Although rigorous high‐quality evaluation of outcomes in social work education is still in the early stages of development (Carpenter,  2011 ), teaching communication skills to social work students is an aspect of the curriculum which has attracted considerable attention, therefore a review of the findings can help to clarify what we know about this important topic.

2.4. Why it is important to do this review

A variety of communication skills courses have been proposed and are in use in social work education. It is nearly twenty years since a number of practice and knowledge reviews highlighted the lack of evaluation into communication skills courses, an issue which warranted further research (Dinham,  2006 ; Trevithick et al.,  2004 ). To support this endeavour, methodological guidance for evaluating outcomes in social work education (Carpenter,  2005 ,  2011 ) has been produced. Consequently, a number of empirical studies (Koprowska,  2010 ; Lefevre,  2010 ; Tompsett, Henderson, Gaskell Mew, et al.,  2017 ) have sought to evaluate the teaching of communication skills among social work students, or investigate the impact of particular components of the intervention. Existing literature suggests that teaching social work students communication skills increases their self‐efficacy in terms of communicative abilities (Koprowska,  2010 ; Lefevre,  2010 ; Tompsett et al.,  2017 ). Good communication is fundamental to effective social work practice.

No comprehensive systematic review or meta‐analysis of this aspect of social work education has been undertaken; questions concerning whether the teaching of communication skills to social work students is effective and produces positive outcomes remain unanswered. It is time therefore to identify, summarise and synthesise the empirical research into a systematic review. Doing so will form a reliable, scientifically rigorous, and accessible account that can be used by educators and policy‐makers to guide decisions about which approaches are effective in teaching communication skills to social work students. In this time of political uncertainty and financial constraint, ‘it is important to accumulate evidence of the outcomes of social work education so that policy‐makers and the public can be confident that it is producing high‐quality social workers’ (Carpenter,  2016 , p. 192), who are suitably equipped to deal with the demands of social work practice. We conducted this systematic review to determine whether CST for social work students works and which types of CST, if any, were the most effective and lead to the most positive outcomes. To improve uptake and relevance, the systematic review was developed in consultation with stakeholders (including academics, students, practitioners, and people with lived experience) and advice was sought from leading social work organisations. The review also sheds light on areas where more research is required.

3. OBJECTIVES

This systematic review aimed to critically evaluate all studies which have investigated the effectiveness of CST programmes for social work students. The PICO (Population, Intervention, Comparator, Outcomes) framework and stakeholder collaboration informed the development of the research question. Student social workers constituted the population, CST was the intervention under investigation, the absence of CST or a course unrelated to communication were the comparators, and attitudes, knowledge, confidence and behavioural changes were the outcomes of interest. Stakeholders had agreed that neither the comparator nor the outcomes should be specified within the research question itself, on the grounds that researchers and academics were unlikely to have specified these elements in the primary studies. The review built on an existing knowledge review (conducted by Trevithick et al.,  2004 ) but was not restricted by the year of publication or language. The research question which the review posed is: ‘What is the effectiveness of CST for improving the communicative abilities of social work students?’ It was intended that the review would provide a robust evaluation of CST for social work students and explain variations in practice. To test the effectiveness of interventions, hierarchies of evidence point to systematic reviews of (preferably randomised) controlled trials. Therefore, we sought to conduct a rigorous and systematic review of such studies about CST, supporting educators and policy‐makers to make evidence‐based decisions in social work education, practice and policy.

The protocol for this review was published in the Campbell Collaboration Library (Reith‐Hall & Montgomery,  2019 ).

4.1. Criteria for considering studies for this review

4.1.1. types of studies.

The studies were required to include an appropriate comparator to be eligible for inclusion in the review, irrespective of whether outcome data were reported in a useable way. Permitted study designs included: randomised trials, non‐randomised trials, controlled before‐after studies, repeated measures studies and interrupted time series studies. To be included, interrupted time series studies needed a clearly defined point in time when the intervention occurred and at least three data points before and three after the intervention. The justification for this wider range of study types was to identify any potential risk of harm which we hoped to assess through wider evidence. Potential risk of harm included any negative effects of CST on students’ communicative abilities, for example, service users and carers might have indicated that students’ poor communication left them feeling more confused, agitated, misunderstood or distressed (i.e., worse) than they did before the interaction.

To ensure quality of evaluation, all studies were critically appraised and an analysis of the results by study design was considered. The comparison group were composed of those who received no educational intervention or those receiving educational interventions other than CST. Trials comparing the effects of two different educational interventions to improve communication skills were also included in this review. In accordance with Campbell policies and guidelines (The Campbell Collaboration,  2014 ), studies without comparison groups or appropriate counterfactual conditions were excluded.

4.1.2. Types of participants

All social work students who were taught communication skills on a generic qualifying social work course in a university setting were included hence undergraduate and postgraduate students were among the types of participants. Students on post‐qualifying courses were excluded.

4.1.3. Types of interventions

Only studies in which the intervention group received CST and in which the control group received nothing or received an alternative training to the intervention group were included. For the intervention, any underpinning theoretical model and any mode of teaching (taught input, videotape recording, role‐play with peers, simulated interviews with service users and carers or actors) were considered acceptable. Interventions that took place either entirely or predominantly in a university setting were included.

4.1.4. Types of outcome measures

Outcomes included changes in (1) knowledge, (2) attitudes, (3) confidence/self‐efficacy and (4) behaviours measured using objective and subjective scales. It was anticipated that these measures might be study‐specific rating scales, developed for use in evaluating communication skills. Stakeholder involvement indicated that behavioural change was an important outcome for all stakeholders. In addition, students and educators deemed confidence/self‐efficacy to be a relevant outcome. In keeping with the literature on outcomes in social work education (Carpenter,  2005 ,  2011 ), student satisfaction alone was not considered as an outcome measure in this review.

4.2. Search methods for identification of studies

We conducted a search for published and unpublished studies using a comprehensive search strategy informed by the guide to information retrieval for Campbell systematic reviews (Kugley et al.,  2017 ). We also sought advice from information specialists. Our search strategy included searching multiple electronic databases, research registers, grey literature sources, and reference lists of prior reviews and relevant studies. Study selection was not restricted by geography, language, publication date or publication status. The original search took place in September 2019 and an updated search took place in June 2021.

4.2.1. Electronic searches

To identify eligible studies the following data sources were searched using the search strings set out in Supporting Information: Appendix  A :

  • (a) Education Abstracts (EBSCO)
  • (b) ERIC (EBSCO)
  • (c) MEDLINE (OVID)
  • (d) PsycINFO (OVID)
  • (e) Web of Science/Knowledge Database Social Science Citation Index
  • (f) Social Services Abstracts (Proquest)
  • (g) ASSIA—Applied Social Sciences Index and Abstracts (Proquest)

Relevant reviews were searched for in the following databases:

  • (i) Database of Abstracts of Reviews of Effectiveness
  • (j) The Campbell Library
  • (k) Cochrane Collaboration Library

We also searched grey literature, using the following databases and websites:

  • (m) Google Scholar—using a series of searches, the first 2 pages of results for each search were screened
  • (n) ProQuest Dissertations and Theses

4.2.2. Searching other resources

We searched for conference proceedings and abstracts through Web of Science and ERIC, followed by a Scopus search which did not unearth any new sources of information. We also looked at generic websites including Google and Bing as well as government websites and professional websites such as gov.uk and the department for education, the Higher Education Academy, British, Australian and American Councils/Associations of Social Work and Social Work Education, Community Care and the Social Care Institute for Excellence website, which includes Social Care online. Several searches containing the key words used in the database searches were replicated for these additional sources.

We also searched the reference lists of the included studies and relevant reviews to identify additional studies. Prominent authors were contacted for further information about their studies and asked if they were aware of any other published or ongoing studies meeting our inclusion criteria. In addition, a final step towards the end of analysis, a manual search of the most recent issue(s) of five key journals that provided relevant studies were identified and checked. These were the Journal of Social Work Education, Social Work Education, the British Journal of Social Work, Children and Youth Services Review and Research on Social Work Practice.

4.3. Data collection and analysis

We collected and analysed data according to our protocol (Reith‐Hall & Montgomery,  2019 ).

One reviewer (ERH) conducted the database searches, removing duplicates and irrelevant records. Having anticipated that the searches would result in very few records to screen, each record was screened by title and abstract by both reviewers (ERH and PM), to ensure robustness. Any studies deemed to be potentially eligible were retrieved in full text and screened by both reviewers. There were no disagreements hence discussions with an arbitrator was not required and consensus was reached in all cases.

The search strategy was developed using the terms featuring in existing knowledge and practice reviews and in consultation with social work researchers and academics, to ensure that the broad range of terminology was included. Search strings included terms relating to the intervention and population but not study design. A sample search strategy for Medline can be found in Supporting Information: Appendix  A . Search strings and search limits were modified for each database. Proximity searching was not required.

4.3.1. Selection of studies

Included studies were any form of design where appropriate counterfactual conditions were satisfied, in accordance with the Cochrane Effective Practice and Organisation of Care guidelines for the inclusion of non‐randomised studies (Cochrane EPOC,  2017 ).

To ensure that the effects of an individual intervention were only counted once, we anticipated applying the following conventions: (1) Where there were multiple measures reported for the same outcome, an average effect size for each outcome would be calculated within each study. (2) Where the same outcome construct was measured across multiple time domains, the main analysis would focus on synthesising the evidence relating to effect sizes at immediate post‐test. Any subsequent measures of outcomes beyond immediate post‐test would be analysed and reported separately.

4.3.2. Data extraction and management

Once eligible studies were found, an initial analysis of intervention descriptions was undertaken for each. The Campbell data collection template form was used to identify the core components of programmes and to develop an overarching typology and coding frame.

Details of study coding categories

Components included:

  • Duration and intensity of the programme.
  • Whether programme delivery included people with lived experience (e.g., service users and carers)
  • Whether programmes used audio and video recording
  • Whether communication skills were practised with peers, service users or actors
  • Whether programmes included observation of social workers in practice
  • The theoretical frameworks underpinning the intervention

Alongside extracting data on programme components, descriptive information for each of the studies was extracted and coded to allow for potential sensitivity and subgroup analysis. This included information regarding:

  • Study characteristics in relation to design, sample sizes, measures and attrition rates.
  • Whether the study was conducted by a research team associated with the programme or an independent team.
  • Stage of programme development, for example whether it was a new programme being piloted or an established programme being replicated.
  • Participants’ characteristics in relation to age, sex, ethnicity, geo‐political region and socio‐economic background.

We considered subgrouping the different types of intervention and population, based on factors such as length of course and teaching methods, age and sex, however the small number of included studies did not warrant subgroup analysis.

Coding was carried out by the review team independently; discrepancies were discussed, and a consensus reached.

Quantitative data was extracted to allow for calculation of effect sizes (using mean change scores and post‐test means and standard deviations). Data was extracted for the intervention and control groups on the relevant outcomes measured to assess the intervention effects.

4.3.3. Assessment of risk of bias in included studies

Assessment of methodological quality and potential for bias was conducted using the Cochrane Risk of Bias tool for randomised studies (Higgins et al.,  2019 ) and the ROBINS‐I tool for non‐randomised studies (Sterne, Higgins, et al.,  2016 ; Sterne, Hernán, et al.,  2016 ).

4.3.4. Measures of treatment effect

Continuous outcomes were reported by the included studies, so we used the standardised mean difference (SMD) as our effect size metric where means and standardised deviations were provided by study authors. Where means and standard deviations were not available, we calculated SMDs from t ‐values and calculated standard deviations from standard errors where these were provided using recommended methods (Higgins et al.,  2022 ). Hedges’ g was used for estimating SMDs to correct for the bias associated with small sample sizes. In studies with more than two groups, we calculated effect sizes using the experimental and control groups that were most relevant to answering our research question or used data from groups with the largest numbers in them.

Treatment of qualitative research

This systematic review was limited to synthesising the available evidence on the effectiveness of CST to social work students. It was beyond the remit of this present review to synthesise the associated evidence related to process evaluations of such programmes hence we did not include qualitative research.

4.3.5. Unit of analysis issues

The unit of analysis for this review was social work students. No unit of analysis issues were identified for the included studies.

4.3.6. Dealing with missing data

Study authors were contacted and accompanying or linked papers were sought in an effort to retrieve missing data.

4.3.7. Assessment of heterogeneity

Widespread clinical heterogeneity within the included studies rendered other anticipated measures of treatment effect non‐viable. For example, the included populations consisted of undergraduate, postgraduate, mixed and unreported students, whilst the interventions differed according to duration, uptake, mode and key features. Widespread methodological diversity was present in terms of designs, methodologies, and outcome measures across studies.

4.3.8. Assessment of reporting biases

Reporting was generally poor among the included studies as evidenced by limited use of reporting instruments such as CONSORT and no references to pre‐published protocols were made by study authors. A more detailed discussion of this issue can be found in the Risk of Bias section. Use of a funnel plot, which helps to identify potential reporting bias in the included studies was not feasible, given the small number of studies included in this review.

The use of a highly sensitive and inclusive systematic search of bibliographic databases, grey literature sources, reference list searching, correspondence with study authors and hand searching sought to counteract potential bias in our reporting of this review.

4.3.9. Data synthesis

As a result of this heterogeneity, meta‐analysis was not feasible, nor was it possible to implement methods outlined in the protocol, such as sensitivity and subgroup analysis. I 2 and Tau 2 were not measured or reported in this review. Similarly, we were unable to use the new GRADE Guidance for Complex Interventions (unpublished) to summarise the overall quality of evidence relating to the primary outcomes.

4.3.10. Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity

N/a in view of there being no meta‐analysis.

4.3.11. Sensitivity analysis

Summary of findings and assessment of the certainty of the evidence, 5.1. description of studies.

There are 15 studies included in this review. An overview of the key characteristics of the included studies, which are described in terms of study design, participants, interventions, comparators, outcomes, outcome measures, geographical location, publication status and implementation factors are provided in Table  1 .

Included studies table.

First author, dateStudy design PopulationInterventionComparatorOutcomesMeasuresLocationPublication statusImplementation factors (e.g., amount, duration)

Barber, 

Experiment 1

Case control32

Undergraduate social work students

Male,  = 8 (25%)

Age range: 19‐46

Mean age: 25.7

Microskills training

 = 16 final year students

No training

 = 16 first year students

Counsellor:

Trustworthiness

Attractiveness

Expertness

Non‐verbal communication

Counselor Rating Form (Barak & LaCrosse,  ).

Non‐verbal rating skills

La Trobe, Victoria, AustraliaPublished journal article

Amount: ‘extensive’

Duration: 4‐year programme

Barber, 

Experiment 2

Case control50

Undergraduate social work students

Population characteristics not stated

Microskills training

 = 25 final year students

No training

 = 25 first year students

Trustworthiness

Attractiveness

Expertness

Counselor Rating Form (Barak & LaCrosse,  ).

Non‐verbal rating skills

La Trobe, Victoria, Australia

Amount: ‘extensive’

Duration: 4‐year programme

Collins,  Case control67Masters level social work students

Skills lab training course (  = 54)

Age range: 21–43

Mean age: 26.78

Male,  = 9 (17%)

Lecture‐based training course (  = 13)

Male,  = 6 (46%)

Interviewing skills; empathy, warmth, genuineness

Skills acquisition measure (SAM)

Carkhuff's communication index

Analogue interview

Client interview

University of Toronto, CanadaDissertation thesis

Amount: not stated

Duration: 2 months

Greeno,  RCT54

Undergraduate and master's level social work students

Male,  = 8 (15%)

51% (  = 28) Caucasian

45% (  = 24) Black

4% (  = 2) Hispanic

Age range: 20–55

Mean age: 29.7

Live supervision with standardised clientsTAU—online self‐study

Perceived empathy

Empathic behaviour

Motivational Interviewing Treatment Integrity; Toronto Empathy QuestionnaireUniversity of Maryland, Baltimore, MD, USAPublished journal article

Amount: 3 days (6 h of didactic teaching, followed by 2 days of Live Supervision or 2 days of online learning)

Duration: unstated (study took place over 7 months, which included 5‐month follow‐up

Pecukonis, 

MI skills, adherent behaviours and proficiency level

Self‐efficacy

Motivational Interviewing Treatment Integrity coding system; general self‐efficacy scalePublished journal article
Hettinga,  RCT38

Masters social work students (  = 34)

Undergraduate students (  = 1)

Male,  = 7 (18%)

Age range: 22‐45

Mean age: 31.3

Communication skills training using videotaped interview playback with instructional feedback  = 23 (3 did not complete measures)Communication skills training (face to face) with group feedback  = 15

Self‐esteem

Self‐perceived interviewing competence

Rosenberg Self‐Esteem Scale

Self‐Perceived Interviewing Competence Questionnaire

University of Minnesota, USADissertation thesis

Amount: 3 h per week

Duration: 1 quarter of an academic year

Keefe,  Case control56

Second year master's social work students

Population characteristics not stated.

(1) a course of instruction with both experiential and didactic content  = 19

(2) a structured meditation series  = 20.

TAU

 = 17

Empathic skillKagan's Affective Sensitivity ScaleThe University of UtahPublished journal article

Instruction

Amount: 2.5 h per week

Duration: 1 quarter of an academic year

Zen meditation

Amount: 30 min per day

Duration: 3 weeks

Larsen,  RCT94

First year master's social work students

Population characteristics not stated.

Communication laboratories consisting of didactic and experiential learning  = 59

Traditional didactic instruction

 = 35

Facilitative conditions (empathy, non‐possessive warmth, genuineness)The Index of Therapeutic CommunicationThe University of UtahPublished journal article

Amount: 10 h

Duration: Not stated

Laughlin,  RCT78

Undergraduate social work students

Male:  = 11 (14.1%)

Age range: 20‐59

Mean age: 23.4

Median age of 21.

(1) Experimental Group I: self‐instruction manual plus audio practice tapes with supervisor evaluation, feedback, and reinforcement

(2) Experimental Group II: self‐instruction manual plus audio practice tapes with self‐evaluation and self‐reinforcement

(3) Control Group I: introductory section of self‐instruction manual, expectation set, and instructions to practice

(4) Control Group II: no instructional materials.

Revised version of the Carkhuff Communication Index (Carkhuff,  ).

Carkhuff's empathic understanding scale (Carkhuff,  )

University of California at Berkeley.Dissertation thesis

Amount: Not stated but experimental groups participated in 3 lab sessions.

Duration: 2 weeks

Ouellette,  Case control30

Undergraduate social work students

Male,  = 2 (6.7%)

Age range: 20–40+

Mean age: Not specified

Age 20–29, 53.3%

Age 30 and 39, 30%

Age 40+, 16.6%

60% (  =   = 18) Caucasian

33.% (  = 10) African American

3.3% (  = 1) Hispanic

3.3% (  = 1) ‘Other’.

Online  =  16Classroom  = 14Basic interviewing skillsBasic practice interviewing skills scaleIndiana UniversityPublished journal article

Amount: 1 × 3‐h session per week

Duration: 15 weeks

Rawlings,  Case control32

Undergraduate social work students

Male,  = 2 (6.3%)

Age range: Not specified

Mean age: 20.81

78% (  = 25) Caucasian

10% (  = 3) Hispanic

6% (  = 2) Biracial

3% (  = 1) African American

3% (  = 1) ‘Other’.

Exiting social work students (  = 16)Starting SW students (  = 16)

Self‐efficacy

Skill performance

Social Work Direct Practice Self‐Efficacy Scale basic practice skill performance (Chang & Scott,  )

Three item direct practice skill sub‐scale reflecting core conditions for each student

Case Western Reserve UniversityDissertation

Amount: not stated

Duration: BSW degree

Schinke,  RCT23

Graduate social work students

Males  = 7 (30.4%)

Age range: Not stated

Mean age: 29.87

Interviewing skills

 = 12

Delayed start control group

 = 11

Attitudes towards their own role‐played interviewing behaviour

Videotaped interview ratings

Counselor effectiveness scale developed by Ivey (1971)

University of WashingtonPublished journal article

Amount: 4 h

Duration one‐off session

Toseland,  Case control68

Undergraduate social work students (  = 55)

Undergraduate social welfare students (  = 13)

Population characteristics not stated.

Interpersonal skills training (  = 55)13 social welfare students—no skills training (  = 13)Ten interpersonal helping skillsThe Carkhuff Indices of Communication and Discrimination and the Counseling Skills Evaluation Parts 1 and 2Not statedPublished journal article

Amount: 15 × 2 sessions in the lab plus lectures (Total of 45 h)

Duration: one semester

VanCleave,  Case control45

Masters level social work students

Age range: early twenties to mid fifties

Mean age: Not stated.

Age 20‐25, 35% (  = 16)

Age 26‐30, 27% (  = 12)

Age 31‐35, 11% (  = 5)

Age 35+, 27% (  = 12)

Male  = 3 (6.6%)

95% (  = 43) Caucasian, 2% (  = 1) African American

2% (  = 1) Japanese

Additional empathy training (  = 22)TAU (  = 23)

Empathic response

Perspective taking and empathic concern

Carkhuff's Index for Communication scripts (CIC)

A 14‐question, self‐survey for Empathic Concern (EC) and the Perspective Taking (PT) subscales of the Davis ( ) Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI).

University of Southern IndianaDissertation thesis

Amount: 10 h

Duration: within a 3‐month cycle

Vinton,  Case control62

Undergraduate social work students

Age range: 19–54

Mean age: 25.9

Male  = 7 (11.3%)

Videotape other

Videotape other + self

Delayed start control group

Perceived empathy

Empathy

Questionnaire Measure of Emotional Empathy

(QMEE)

Carkhuff's level of empathy scale

Florida State UniversityPublished journal article

Amount: not stated but includes a 100‐min standardised lecture.

Duration: not stated

Wells,  RCT14

Social work students (type not specified)

Population characteristics not stated.

Role‐playOwn problemsEmpathyCarkhuff's empathic understanding scaleUniversity of PittsburghPublished journal article

Amount: 1 day of didactic training, 6 × 2‐h sessions of experiential learning

Duration: not stated

5.2. Results of the search

The main bibliographic database and registers search, completed in September 2019, returned 1998 records with an additional 12 added after the search was updated in June 2021. After 882 duplicate records were removed, 1128 were subjected to initial screening by title, and abstract if necessary, following which a further 1021 records were removed because they were not relevant to the topic. Of the 107 remaining records, 2 could not be retrieved despite endeavours to locate them through different libraries and searches, therefore 105 records were fully screened for eligibility, 9 of which met the inclusion criteria.

Another 650 studies were identified through recent editions of five key journals identified through the database search. A further 19 studies were identified through other methods including citation searching within the included studies. Of the 669 studies subjected to initial screening, 627 were removed because they were not relevant to the topic. One record could not be retrieved resulting in 41 records being fully screened for eligibility, of which 34 records were excluded, and 7 records (reporting 6 studies) were included.

Of the fifteen studies which met the inclusion criteria for this systematic review, two experiments are reported in a single paper (Barber,  1988 ), one study is reported in two papers (Greeno et al.,  2017 ; Pecukonis et al.,  2016 )‐with both authors contributing to the write‐up of each, and another study (Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ) is also written up as the first author's PhD thesis (Larsen,  1975 ).

The search results are shown in the PRISMA diagram (adapted from Page et al.,  2021 ) in Figure  1 .

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is CL2-19-e1309-g001.jpg

PRISMA diagram.

5.2.1. Included studies

Study design characteristics.

Despite the varied terminology used by the study authors to describe their research designs, eight reports, addressing nine studies (Barber,  1988 ; Collins,  1984 ; Keefe,  1979 ; Ouellette et al.,  2006 ; Rawlings,  2008 ; Toseland & Spielberg,  1982 ; VanCleave,  2007 ; Vinton & Harrington,  1994 ) employed a case‐controlled design, some of which conform to the parameters of a pre‐experimental static group comparison design (Campbell & Stanley,  1963 ). This means that participants were divided between two groups but in a non‐randomised way. Given students were not randomised to the different groups, these studies suffer from weak internal validity, with confounders such as maturation, the Hawthorne effect, testing effects and pre‐existing differences between the intervention and control groups. Such issues are common in educational research.

Six of the studies reported in seven papers were randomised controlled trials (RCTs), five of which were conducted in the mid to late 1970s. The increase of research activity surrounding this topic during this decade likely results from the development of teaching models such as Ivey and Authier's micro‐counselling model (Ivey & Authier,  1971 ; Ivey et al.,  1968 ) and the Truax and Carkhuff Human Relations training model (Carkhuff,  1969c ; Truax & Carkhuff,  1967 ), alongside the development of research measures, including the Carkhuff scales (Carkhuff,  1969a ,  1969b ), which are the most cited research instrument in this review.

Wells ( 1976 ) is the earliest of the included studies to use an RCT design, comparing role‐play and students’ ‘own problem’ procedures, but the sample size contained just 14 students. Hettinga ( 1978 ) had a somewhat larger sample of 38 students, in which immediate feedback from an instructor was compared with group feedback provided later. Although quasi‐randomisation took place, it is unlikely the allocation method affected the results. In the same year, Larsen and Hepworth ( 1978 ) investigated the role of experiential learning; controls received traditional didactic instruction. Schinke et al. ( 1978 ) randomly allocated a group of 23 students to either an intervention group or a waiting‐list control. Laughlin ( 1978 ) used a more complex design consisting of two experimental groups and two control groups. Despite using pre‐tests, a strategy which can help overcome methodological challenges associated with small sample sizes (social work cohorts are typically small), the study was hopelessly underpowered. The most recent of the included studies, reported in the two papers by Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) and Greeno et al. ( 2017 ), offers the most robust research design of the included studies. Not only did they exceed the minimum sample size calculated in an a priori power analysis, but the overall risk of bias was lower than other studies included in this review.

In terms of comparators, in four of the studies the control group received no intervention (Barber,  1988 ; Rawlings,  2008 ; Toseland & Spielberg,  1982 ); three studies reported controls receiving treatment as usual (TAU) (Greeno et al.,  2017 ; Keefe,  1979 ; Pecukonis et al.,  2016 ; VanCleave,  2007 ), however the TAU in Greeno and Pecukonis’ study was an online intervention, as opposed to an absence of an intervention; and a further five studies compared two different interventions. These included an experiential approach with traditional didactic learning (Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ); lab‐based versus lecture‐based training (Collins,  1984 ); online versus classroom‐based teaching (Ouellette et al.,  2006 ); videotaped interview playback with instructional feedback versus peer group feedback (Hettinga,  1978 ); and role‐play versus students’ ‘own problems’ procedures (Wells,  1976 ). In a rather complex design, Laughlin  1978 study included two treatment arms, and two control groups, one of which received no treatment. In two subsequent studies (Schinke et al.,  1978 ; Vinton & Harrington,  1994 ), the controls had a delayed start (operating as a waiting list procedure).

Significant issues with measurement are evident within the included studies and are acknowledged by several of the researchers (Collins,  1984 ; Greeno et al.,  2017 ; Laughlin,  1978 ; Vinton & Harrington,  1994 ). Methodological challenges will be considered in Section  6 .

Publication status

Five of the studies were dissertation theses (Collins,  1984 ; Hettinga,  1978 ; Laughlin,  1978 ; Rawlings,  2008 ; VanCleave,  2007 ), with the remainder being reported in peer reviewed journals (Barber,  1988 ; Greeno et al.,  2017 ; Keefe,  1979 ; Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ; Ouellette et al.,  2006 ; Pecukonis et al.,  2016 ; Schinke et al.,  1978 ; Toseland & Spielberg,  1982 ; Vinton & Harrington,  1994 ; Wells,  1976 ).

Population characteristics

A total of 743 research participants were contained within the 15 included studies. Of the included studies, seven studies (reported in Barber,  1988 ; Laughlin,  1978 ; Ouellette et al.,  2006 ; Rawlings,  2008 ; Toseland & Spielberg,  1982 ; Vinton & Harrington,  1994 ) contained undergraduate students ( N  = 352) and five studies (Collins,  1984 ; Keefe,  1979 ; Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ; Schinke et al.,  1978 ; VanCleave,  2007 ) comprised Master's social work students as their participants ( N  = 285). One study (Wells,  1976 ) failed to specify student type ( N  = 14) whilst two studies (Greeno et al.,  2017 ; Hettinga,  1978 ; Pecukonis et al.,  2016 ) used a combination of undergraduate and Master's students ( N  = 92).

Ten of the included studies report on the number and percentage of men and women in the student samples. In Collins' ( 1984 ) study, of the 54 students in the lab group, 17% ( N  = 9) were men, however of the 13 students from the lecture group sample, 46% ( N  = 6) were men; the number of men in the lecture group was unusually high. Collins ( 1984 , p. 74) acknowledges this is not explained by the admissions procedures at either of the universities involved in the study. However, it must be remembered that the 13 students from the lecture group, who volunteered to be part of the study, are not necessarily representative of the cohort demographic.

A more consistent picture is evident amongst the other studies, in which men make up less than a third of the social work students in the samples, reflecting a demographic pattern found among qualified social workers. The number and percentage of men in the student samples (arranged in ascending order by percentage) were as follows: 6% ( N  = 2) for Rawlings ( 2008 ); almost 7% for both Ouellette et al. ( 2006 ) and VanCleave ( 2007 ) ( N  = 2 and N  = 3, respectively); 11% ( N  = 7) for Vinton and Harrington ( 1994 ); 14% ( N  = 11) for Laughlin ( 1978 ); 15% ( N  = 8) in the study reported by Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) and Greeno et al. ( 2017 ); 18% ( N  = 7) for Hettinga ( 1978 ); 25% ( N  = 8) in Barber ( 1988 ) ‐ experiment 1 and just over 30% ( N  = 7) in the study conducted by Schinke et al. ( 1978 ). The sex of students was not reported in five of the studies (Barber,  1988 ‐experiment 2; Keefe,  1979 ; Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ; Toseland & Spielberg,  1982 ; Wells,  1976 ).

Due to differences in reporting practices, the age characteristics of the students in the included studies are harder to compare. In the same five studies identified above (Barber,  1988 ‐experiment 2; Keefe,  1979 ; Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ; Toseland & Spielberg,  1982 ; Wells,  1976 ), age characteristics were not reported.

The age range was not specified in Rawlings' ( 2008 ) study, although students had the lowest mean age of 20.8 (18.8 for entering students and 22.9 for exiting students). The mean age of students in Laughlin's study was 23.4, with the broadest age ranges of 20‐59. In Barber's ( 1988 ) paper, for experiment 1 the ages ranged from 19 to 46 years, with a mean age of 25.7 years. With a slightly broader age range of 19–54, students in Vinton and Harrington's ( 1994 ) study had a mean age of 25.9. In Collins' ( 1984 ) study, the ages of the lab trained students ranged from 21 to 43 years, with a mean age of 26.7; the lecture trained students are described as being ‘slightly older’ (p. 74). The age range for the students in the study reported by Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) and Greeno et al. ( 2017 ) was 20–55, with a mean age of 29.7. An age range was not specified in Schinke et al's. ( 1978 ) study, although the mean age was 29.87. Of the studies where data about mean age were available, students in the study undertaken by Hettinga,  1978 had the oldest mean age of 31.3, with an age range of 22–45. In Ouellette et al's. ( 2006 ) study, an age range of 20–40+ is reported. A mean age is not provided, however 53.3% of students were between the ages of 20 and 29, 30% were between the ages of 30 and 39, and 16.6% were older than 40. In keeping with the age ranges of the other studies, the age range in VanCleave's ( 2007 ) study was described as early twenties to mid‐fifties. No mean age was provided, however 35% ( N  = 16) of students were between the ages of 20 and 25, almost 27% ( N  = 12) were between 26 and 30, 11% ( N  = 5) were between the ages of 31 to 35 and almost 27% ( N  = 12) were over 35 years.

Only the four studies conducted since 2000 reported information on ethnicity, in the following ways: In the study conducted by Ouellette et al. ( 2006 ), 60% ( N  = 18) of students were Caucasian, 33.3% ( N  = 10) were African American, 3.3% ( N  = 1) were Hispanic, and 3.3% ( N  = 1) identified as ‘Other’. Rawlings ( 2008 ) identified that 78% of students ( N  = 25) were Caucasian, almost 10% ( N  = 3) were Hispanic, just over 6% ( N  = 2) were Biracial, 3% ( N  = 1) were African American and 3% ( N  = 1) were defined as ‘Other’. In the study reported by Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) and Greeno et al. ( 2017 ), just over 51% ( N  = 28) of students were Caucasian, 45% ( N  = 24) were Black and almost 4% ( N  = 2) were Hispanic. In VanCleave's ( 2007 ) study, over 95% ( N  = 43) of students were Caucasian, one student was African American and one was Japanese—each accounting for just over 2%. The earlier studies did not report on the ethnicities of their participants, reflecting changes to trends in the collection of demographic data.

Data is absent for other demographic characteristics within the included studies.

Location characteristics

There is little variation within the geo‐political contexts in which the included studies were conducted. This is important because it reflects some priorities such as the primacy placed on experimental design, at the expense of others, including stakeholder involvement. One study, Collins ( 1984 ) ( N  = 67) was undertaken in Toronto, Canada, whilst Barber ( 1988 ) reports on two experiments conducted in Victoria, Australia ( N  = 82). One study, Toseland and Spielberg ( 1982 ) did not provide a location ( N  = 68). The remaining 11 studies were carried out in different US states, where the focus on evidence‐based teaching and learning in social work education is firmly established. Involvement and participation from people with lived experience was noticeably absent—the second of the Barber ( 1988 ) experiments and the client interviews in Collins' ( 1984 ) study being the exceptions. None of the included studies were conducted in the UK, where a strong tradition of service user and carer involvement in social work education prevails, which arguably explains, but does not justify, the omission of contributions from people with lived experience within the body of research identified in this review.

Intervention characteristics

Theoretical orientation.

Experiential learning is referred to in the majority of the studies (Collins,  1984 ; Greeno et al.,  2017 ; Keefe,  1979 ; Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ; Laughlin,  1978 ; Pecukonis et al.,  2016 ; Rawlings,  2008 ; Schinke et al.,  1978 ; Toseland & Spielberg,  1982 ) as the underpinning theoretical orientation of the intervention under investigation. However, the term is not applied consistently. With its wide range of different meanings, ideologies, methods and practices, experiential learning is conceptually complex and difficult to define (Moon,  1999 ). Conceptualisations arising from two different traditions are evident within the included studies: first, the work of Carkhuff and Truax ( 1965 ) and Ivey and Authier ( 1971 ), which derive from psychotherapy, and second, the work of Kolb ( 1984 ) and Schön ( 1987 ) which is grounded in a constructivist view of education and has been particularly instrumental within professional courses.

Although deriving from psychotherapy, the microskills counselling approach developed by Ivey et al. ( 1968 ) and Ivey and Authier ( 1971 ) has informed the teaching of interviewing skills in social work education. Content comprises well‐defined counselling skills including attending behaviour, minimal activity responses, and verbal following behaviour. Six of the included studies made reference to the work of Ivey and colleagues, however five of them (Collins,  1984 ; Hettinga,  1978 ; Laughlin,  1978 ; Rawlings,  2008 ; VanCleave,  2007 ) did so simply within a discussion of the wider literature. It is only in Schinke et al.'s ( 1978 ) study where Ivey's work has a direct impact on the empirical evaluation itself; an adapted version of the Counsellor Effectiveness Scale developed by Ivey and Authier ( 1971 ) was used as one of the study's measuring instruments.

Referred to as the Human Relations training model, the work of Carkhuff and Truax ( 1965 ) and Carkhuff ( 1969c ) has been more influential than Ivey's approach. A brief exploration of empathy as a theoretical construct helps to explain why Carkhuff and Truax's work has influenced social work education and practice. Whilst linguistic relevance can be seen in the Greek word ‘empatheia’, which means appreciation of another's pain, the philosophical underpinnings of the term empathy actually derives from the German word Einfühlung. Theodor Lipps expanded the conceptualisation of empathy to include the notion of minded creatures, of which inner resonance and imitation are a part. Lipps’ ideas influenced how empathy came to be understood in psychotherapy and is evident in the work of Sigmund Freud and Carl Rogers. Empathy was identified by Rogers ( 1957 ) as one of ‘the necessary and sufficient conditions’ for therapeutic personality change; his ideas about person‐centred practice remain central to social work education and practice today. Charles Truax, a protégé of Rogers, worked closely with Robert Carkhuff, to explore how conceptual orientations such as empathy could be observed, repeated, measured and taught. Carkhuff and Truax ( 1965 ) developed and evaluated an integrated didactic and experiential approach in a counselling and psychotherapy context, which ‘focuses upon therapist development and growth’ (p. 333). Their work, and the ideas that influenced them, are evident throughout the earlier studies of this review where empathy was the focus. Barber ( 1988 ) cited Carkhuff's work on empathy in his discussion of the literature, whilst Keefe ( 1979 ) referred to it for teaching purposes only. Seven studies (Collins,  1984 ; Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ; Laughlin,  1978 ; Toseland & Spielberg,  1982 ; VanCleave,  2007 ; Vinton & Harrington,  1994 ; Wells,  1976 ) used the Carkhuff scales (Carkhuff,  1969a ;  1969b ) as an outcome measure in their empirical research. As identified by Elliott et al. ( 2018 ), the Carkhuff scales were some of the earliest observer measures, which may well explain the popularity of this instrument. The focus the researchers of the included studies placed on empathy is striking and will be considered further in subsequent sections.

Also apparent in the literature is the experiential learning approach deriving from the experiential learning cycle developed by Kolb ( 1984 ) and the concept of reflective practice articulated by Schön ( 1987 ). Rawlings ( 2008 ), who provides the most comprehensive overview of experiential learning in the included studies, draws on the work of both. Huerta‐Wong and Schoech ( 2010 ) suggest that experiential learning has been a teaching technique used extensively to teach social workers skills in the United Kingdom and the United States since the 1990s. They explain that ‘experiential learning proposes that effective learning is influenced by a cycle of experimentation, reflection, research, and exercising’ (Huerta‐Wong & Schoech,  2010 , p. 86), elements of which feature in the body of work comprising this review. The experimentation component is well defined and clearly identifiable. Keefe ( 1979 ) describes highly structured role‐play situations occurring within an experiential learning component. Similarly, in the live supervision intervention reported by Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) and Greeno et al. ( 2017 ), experiential learning opportunities are described as occurring within a small group format, using a one‐way mirror in a classroom setting to practice with standardised clients. VanCleave ( 2007 ) appears to draw on both concepts of experiential learning outlined above: the ‘homework experientials’ featuring in the training intervention comprise a series of practical tasks based on a range of different learning styles, which students complete between sessions to augment the development of empathy. In Ouellette et al.'s ( 2006 ) study, reference is made to the importance of adult learning principles and effective active learning paradigms in technology‐supported instructional environments.

Bandura's propositions are also evident within the included studies. VanCleave ( 2007 ) draws on social learning theory (Bandura,  1971 ), recognising that the modelling of skills is important for learning. Ideas about self‐reinforcement (Bandura,  1976 ) influenced Laughlin ( 1978 ), in a consideration of the impact of internal and external motivation. The exploration into the role of self‐efficacy by Rawlings ( 2008 ) in skill development was informed by self‐efficacy and social cognitive theory (Bandura,  1997 ). Behaviour, according to social cognitive theory, is influenced by goals, outcome expectations, self‐efficacy expectations and socio‐structural determinants (Bandura,  1982 ). Much of the literature indicates the potential impact of students’ self‐efficacy beliefs for the teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education.

Irrespective of which conceptualisation is used, the value of experiential learning has withstood the test of time and is the front runner in terms of the theoretical orientation underpinning the teaching and learning of CST, or specific components of it, both of which are addressed in this review. Toseland and Spielberg ( 1982 ) consider experiential learning fundamental to the systematic training that the teaching of communication skills requires. In a review of practice of teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education in England, Dinham ( 2006 ) identified a strong emphasis on experiential and participative teaching and learning methods.

Other theories, for example ego psychology in Hettinga ( 1978 ) are discussed particularly in the dissertation theses; however, the theoretical orientations underpinning the pedagogical approaches are largely ill‐defined or absent from the outcome studies in this review.

Delivery and approach

The included studies do provide some insight into the delivery format and teaching methods under investigation, especially where studies compare teaching modalities or approaches. A concerning issue in the earlier studies is whether practicing skills in communication and empathy (utilising an experiential component) is more effective than a purely didactic traditional lecture‐based approach. Larsen and Hepworth ( 1978 ) compared the efficacy of a traditional didactic intervention with an experiential intervention used within communication laboratories. Collins ( 1984 ) also compared a lecture‐based training course with a skills lab training course. The results of these studies supported practice‐based experiential learning. By contrast, when Keefe ( 1979 ) compared an experiential‐didactic course to a structured meditation experience with a control group, the experiential group did not make the expected gains, whereas those receiving  meditation did. In an extension of the basic design, Keefe ( 1979 ) found a combination of experiential training and structured meditation proved most effective.

Some of the more current studies focussed on classroom‐based teaching versus online delivery, an issue particularly relevant in the current global pandemic, which in many instances has seen teaching move to purely online or blended delivery. Ouellette et al. ( 2006 ) compared a classroom‐based instructional approach with an online web‐based instructional approach and found no significant differences between the two. In the study reported by Greeno et al. ( 2017 ) and Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) however, live supervision with standardised clients compared favourably with the TAU, which they describe as being online self‐study.

Other studies compared more specific components within the intervention. The role of active learning for students was important whether that included participation in role‐play with peers or simulated clients. Wells ( 1976 ) in comparing the use of roleplay with using participants’ own problems, found neither one proved preferential but identified the active experimentation of students as being the key factor in their interpersonal skills development.

The role of the instructor was also an issue of interest. Hettinga ( 1978 ) examined the benefits of 1:1 instructor feedback compared with small group feedback, Laughlin ( 1978 ) focused on the role of instructor feedback versus self‐evaluation whilst Greeno et al. ( 2017 ) and Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) expressed optimism for the use of live supervision. Again, whilst no claim can be made for whom the feedback provider (self, peers or instructor) should be, active engagement with the evaluation and feedback process seems to be the underlying mechanism which facilitates change. Opportunities for playback was another area for investigation. Reflecting the rapid development of technology in recent years, Laughlin ( 1978 ) investigated the use of audiotapes whereas Vinton and Harrington's ( 1994 ) instructional package consisted of watching videotapes of themselves or others engaging in communicative interactions. Opportunities to observe practice have a facilitative quality, a point recognised by the study authors who drew on Bandura's work.

Although there are not enough studies comparing like for like to draw any firm conclusions, the current body of research indicates that the rehearsal of skills through role‐play or simulation accompanied by opportunities for observation, feedback and reflection offer benefits for systematic CST, facilitating small gains, on skill‐based outcome measures at least. Some of the authors included in this review are confident in recommending specific teaching methods. Toseland and Spielberg ( 1982 ) suggest practice, feedback and modelling are necessary; Schinke et al. ( 1978 ) add role playing, cueing, and positive reinforcement to this list. Greeno et al.'s ( 2017 ) advice to educators is similar, with the added recommendation of supervision. Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) highlighted modelling of techniques to students as key. In a review of empathy training in which meta‐analysis was feasible, Teding van Berkhout and Malouff ( 2015 ) suggest that studies in which behavioural skills were developed through instruction, modelling, practice and feedback had higher, but not significantly higher, effect sizes than those in which some or all of these components were missing. Findings from qualitative research indicate that students learn communication and interviewing skills through the practice, observation, feedback and reflection that accompany simulation and role‐play activities, which Banach et al. ( 2020 ) found mapped onto Kolb's ( 1984 ) model of experiential learning. Further exploration of these issues is required.

Implementation factors: Amount, duration and uptake

Considerable variation in terms of amount and duration is evident across the included studies. The briefest intervention was a single 4‐h training session (Schinke et al.,  1978 ) whilst the longest intervention, described as ‘extensive’ appears to be interspersed throughout a 4‐year degree course (Barber,  1988 ). Literature has documented the ability to teach empathy at a minimally facilitative level in as few as 10 h (Carkhuff,  1969c ; Carkhuff & Berenson,  1976 ; Truax & Carkhuff,  1967 ). Indeed, Larsen and Hepworth ( 1978 ) found positive change occurred from a 10‐h intervention, but ‘estimated that 20 h, preferably 2 h per week for 10 weeks, would be ample’ (p. 79). However, Toseland and Spielberg ( 1982 ) suggested that the course under investigation in their study, which lasted approximately 45 h (30 h of which were experiential learning in a laboratory) may not be sufficient to increase students’ skill to the level of competence expected of a professional worker. In the study undertaken by VanCleave ( 2007 ), implementation of the intervention appeared to vary between students, because ‘when assignment by cohort could not be achieved, training was subdivided into smaller groups. Given the flexibility of the researcher, individual training was accommodated’ (p. 119). It is likely this variation occurred to enhance student participation in the study, maximising data collection opportunities for research purposes.

A number of studies did not report details regarding the amount and duration of the intervention, and some provided rather vague or imprecise details, rendering comparative aims regarding amount and duration of training futile.

The studies focus on what was taught, but data on uptake is sorely lacking. Some of the included studies (Collins,  1984 ; Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ; Ouellette et al.,  2006 ) compared students’ personal and demographic characteristics alongside their pre‐course training and/or experience. The role of sex, age and pre‐course experience were key considerations. Social work courses attract few men compared to women, and often have small cohorts, making judgements on demographic characteristics difficult. Vinton and Harrington ( 1994 ), who examined the impact of sex on students’ empathy levels, found women had higher QMEE scores than men at both pre and post‐test. This is consistent with a study undertaken by Zaleski ( 2016 ) which found female students in medicine, dentistry, nursing, pharmacy, veterinary, and law were found to have higher levels of empathy than their male peers.

Counterintuitively, age was not found to be significantly correlated to communication skills. Ouellette et al. ( 2006 ) queried whether age was a factor in learning, yet summary statements was the only item on their interview rating scale found to be significantly correlated to age. Collins ( 1984 ) found that the amount of prior training had no impact on students’ ability to demonstrate interpersonal skills. Similarly, in a comparison of the mean levels achieved by groups dichotomised on the basis of age, sex, previous social work experience, and undergraduate social welfare or other major, Larsen and Hepworth ( 1978 ) found such attributes yielded no significant differences on either pre‐ or post‐test scores. Both studies challenge the assumption that students with more social care experience before training possess more or better communication skills than those without. In terms of uptake, Larsen and Hepworth ( 1978 , p. 78) suggested that ‘a mix with contrasting skill levels appears advantageous’, because ‘students with higher‐level skills modelled facilitative responses in the practice sessions for students with lower skills, thus encouraging and assisting the latter to achieve higher levels of responding’. In the study conducted by Laughlin ( 1978 ), self‐instruction students exhibited significantly higher mean scores for enjoyment and number of optional practice items completed than students in an instructor‐led group. Self‐instruction ‘creates a sense of self‐reliance, confidence, and personal responsibility for learning which promotes enjoyment and devotion to task not present under circumstances of external control’ (Laughlin,  1978 , p. 67). Self‐instruction appears to facilitate uptake. Other issues affecting student learning such as concentration or care‐giving responsibilities and their impact on uptake were not addressed in any of the studies included in this review.

5.2.2. Excluded studies

There were 33 papers covering 30 studies, which narrowly missed the inclusion criteria, or which content experts might expect to see in the review. There were two main reasons for exclusion, both of which are outlined in the review protocol (Reith‐Hall & Montgomery,  2019 ). First, the study design did not meet the minimum standards of methodological rigour, predominantly because an appropriate comparator was lacking. Second, the population was too specific, drawn from social work courses purely focusing on child welfare or working with children, or too general‐including students drawn from a variety of different courses. A full list of excluded studies and reasons for exclusion is presented in Table  2 .

Excluded studies table.

Author (first)DateReason for exclusion
Andrews2017No comparator
Bakx2006No comparator
Barclay2012No comparator
Bogo2017No intervention
Bolger2014No comparator
Carrillo1993No comparator
Carrillo1994Unsuitable comparator
Carter2018No comparator
Cartney2006No comparator
Cetingok1988Insufficient time points
Collins1987Unsuitable intervention and comparator
Corcoran2019No comparator
Domakin2013No comparator
Gockel2014No comparator
Hansen2002No comparator
Hodorowicz2018Population too specific (child welfare training)
Hodorowicz2020Population too specific (child welfare training)
Hohman2015No comparator
Kopp1982No comparator
Kopp 1985No comparator
Kopp1990No comparator
Koprowska2010Unsuitable comparator
Lefevre2010No comparator
Magill1985No comparator
Mishna2013Unsuitable comparator
Nerdrum1995Population too specific (child care pedagogues)
Nerdrum 1997Population too specific (child care pedagogues)
Nerdrum 2003Population too specific (child care pedagogues)
Patton2020Population too general (psychology & social justice)
Rogers2009No comparator
Scannapieco2000Population too specific (child welfare training)
Tompsett2017Instrument development
Wodarski1988No clear intervention

5.3. Risk of bias in included studies

Both review authors assessed the risk of bias of the included studies, independently applying the ‘Risk of bias’ tools—ROB 2 (Sterne et al.,  2019 ) for the randomised trials and Robins‐I for the non‐randomised studies of interventions (Sterne, Hernán, et al.,  2016 ). Both tools comprise a set of bias domains, intended to cover all issues that might lead to a risk of bias (Boutron et al.,  2021 ). We used the Methodological Expectations of Cochrane Intervention Reviews (MECIR) guidance (Higgins et al.,  2021 ), The Revised Cochrane risk‐of‐bias tool for randomised trials (RoB 2) (Higgins et al.,  2019 ) and the Risk of Bias in Non‐randomised Studies of Interventions (ROBINS‐I): detailed guidance (Sterne, Higgins, et al., 2016) to inform our judgements. To answer the review's research question, we were interested in assessing the effect of assignment to the intervention, as opposed to adherence to the intervention. Discrepancies between review author judgements were resolved through discussion.

Both reviewers judged there to be a moderate or high/serious risk of bias in all but three of 15 included studies, with only one study receiving a low risk of bias rating overall, with an additional two studies receiving a low bias rating overall for one outcome measure but not the other. The lack of information for certain domains was a problem in all of the studies, highlighting that in future, researchers should report a greater level of detail to enable the risk of bias to be fully assessed. Using a tool such as CONSORT SPI (Grant et al.,  2018 ) would facilitate this.

5.3.1. Risk of bias in randomised trials

As shown in Table  3 , there was considerable variation within the risk of bias domains of the non‐randomised studies. Only one study was rated as low risk of bias, one was rated as having ‘some concerns’, three were rated as being at high risk of bias and one study (reported in two papers) received a mix of overall bias ratings, according to the outcomes measured. Limitations were evident in all of the studies, including the lack of information reported in domains 2 and 5.

Risk of bias summary table for randomised studies based on ROB 2.

StudyDomain 1Domain 2Domain 3Domain 4Domain 5Overall risk of bias
Risk of bias arising from the randomisation processRisk of bias due to deviations from the intended interventionsRisk of bias due to missing outcome dataRisk of bias in measurement of the outcomeRisk of bias in selection of the reported result
Hettinga ( )LOWNot reportedHIGHSelf‐perceived skillsNot reportedHIGH
HIGH
Self‐esteem
SOME
Larsen and Hepworth ( )LOWNot reportedLOWLOW Not reportedLOW
Laughlin ( )LOWNot reportedHIGHHIGHNot reportedHIGH
Greeno et al. ( )LOW Not reportedLOWPerceived empathyNot reportedPerceived empathy
SOMESOME
Behaviour changeBehaviour change
LOWLOW
Pecukonis et al. ( )Self‐efficacySelf‐efficacy
SOMESOME
Behaviour changeBehaviour change
LOWLOW
Schinke et al. ( )SOMENot reportedLOWSelf‐perceived skillsNot reportedSOME
SOME
Behaviour change
LOW
Wells ( )SOMEHIGHHIGHLOWNot reportedHIGH

Domain 1—Bias arising from the randomisation process

Randomisation aims to avoid an influence of either known or unknown prognostic factors. There was considerable variation provided by the study authors regarding the randomisation process. Where there was sufficient information about the method of recruitment and allocation to suggest the groups were comparable with respect to prognostic factors (Hettinga,  1978 ; Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ; Laughlin,  1978 ), the risk of bias was considered low. This level of detail is provided by Laughlin ( 1978 ): a table of random numbers ensured allocation sequence generation; manila envelopes were used for allocation sequence concealment; and potential prognostic factors such as age, prior job and training experience were measured as equivalent for all groups at the outset.

Conversely, information required for ROB 2 was missing from the other studies, some of which was gleaned by directly contacting study authors. Elizabeth Greeno provided additional details about the randomisation process, enabling the risk of bias in the study reported by Greeno et al. ( 2017 ) and Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) to be rated as low. Schinke et al. ( 1978 ) and Wells ( 1976 ) stated that students were randomly assigned to groups, however they did not provide any details about how students were recruited or allocated. Both authors have passed away so further information could not be ascertained. Although there were no obvious baseline differences between groups to indicate a problem with the randomisation process, the absence of detailed information led to a judgement of some concern for both studies in this domain.

Domain 2—Risk of bias due to deviations from the intended interventions (effect of assignment to intervention)

Given placebos and sham interventions are generally not feasible in educational interventions, students and staff tended to be aware of which intervention the students were assigned to, particularly since students were largely drawn from cohorts known to each other. Control group scores were markedly different from intervention scores, suggesting contamination between groups did not occur. In reviewing the papers, there were no reports of control groups receiving the active intervention, nor did trialists report that they had changed the intervention. However, a lack of information about deviations from the intended interventions is reflected in our use of the term ‘not reported’.

Similarly, there was no information as to whether an appropriate analysis had been used to estimate the effect of assignment to intervention. Higgins et al. ( 2019 , p. 26) acknowledge that ‘exclusions are often poorly reported, particularly in the pre‐CONSORT era before 1996’. Apart from the study reported by Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) and Greeno et al. ( 2017 ), the randomised trials included in this review were conducted in the 1970s, which helps to explain why making interpretations of the risk of bias for these empirical studies was particularly difficult. For most of the randomised trials, there was nothing to suggest that there was potential for a substantial impact (on the result) of the failure to analyse participants in the group to which they were randomised. However, again a lack of information led the reviewers to replace a bias rating with ‘not reported’. Wells ( 1976 ) study provides an exception to this rule. Noting that two students from each group swapped due to placement clashes, Wells did not perceive this as an issue. However, the data of these students were analysed in terms of the interventions they received rather than the interventions to which they were initially assigned. As a result, both review authors deemed the risk of bias rating to be high for this domain.

Domain 3: Risk of bias due to missing outcome data

Some studies (Greeno et al.,  2017 ; Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ; Pecukonis et al.,  2016 ; and Schinke et al.,  1978 ) retained almost all of their participants hence no data or very little data were missing, warranting a low risk of bias rating for the missing outcome data domain. Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) for example, identify low attrition as a strength in their study, highlighting that retention at T3 and T4 was 96% and 94%, respectively (p. 501).

Three studies were judged to be at high risk of bias due to missing data and a lack of any accompanying information. Laughlin ( 1978 ) identified that out of 68 students in her study, ‘seven subjects failed to complete either the pre‐ or post‐test because of absence from class on the day these tests were administered’ (p. 40). Information about the group for which data were missing was not provided. In Wells' ( 1976 ) study, the four students who were not present at post‐testing were excluded from the analysis, and whilst the number may seem small, they represent a significant proportion of the original study sample, which comprised only 14 students. Hettinga ( 1978 , p. 57) ‘assumes that no interaction of selection and mortality occurred’, yet researcher assumptions do not constitute evidence. In all three of these studies, the reasons for the absences were unclear and there was no evidence to indicate that the result was not biased by missing outcome data. The authors did not discuss whether missingness depended on, or was likely to depend on, its true value. Yet it is possible, likely even, that missingness in the outcome data could be related to the outcome's true value if, for example, students who perceived their communication skills to be poor decided not to attend the post‐test measurements. As a result of this, and the study authors’ lack of attention to these issues, we judged there to be a high risk of bias due to missing outcome data in the trials undertaken by Hettinga ( 1978 ), Laughlin ( 1978 ), and Wells ( 1976 ).

Domain 4: Risk of bias in measurement of the outcome

Randomised trials are judged as low risk of bias in measurement of the outcome if: the methods are deemed appropriate, do not differ between intervention groups, and ensure that independent assessors are blinded to intervention assignment. Wells' ( 1976 ) study explicitly met this criterion. Based on Larsen and Hepworth's ( 1978 ) article, the risk of bias would have been rated conservatively high because the study does not say if the outcome assessors knew to which group the students belonged. However, in her PhD thesis, on which Larsen and Hepworth's ( 1978 ) article is based, Larsen ( 1975 ) clearly states that three social work raters were blind to the identification of the student and to their intervention/control group status. The additional information enabled reviewers to judge this domain as being at low risk of bias.

In studies where two different outcome measures were used, bias ratings were judged separately, indicated by the split outcomes in domain 4 in Table  3 . For Greeno et al. ( 2017 ), Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) and Schinke et al. ( 1978 ), low bias ratings were given for measures of behaviour change due to evidence of independent raters, blind to the intervention status of participants. However, the self‐report measures used by each, warrant a higher risk of bias. According to the Rob 2 guidance, for self‐reported outcomes, the assessment of outcome is potentially influenced by knowledge of the intervention received, leading to a judgement of at least some concerns (Higgins et al.,  2019 , p. 51). If review authors judge it likely that participants’ reporting of the outcome was influenced by knowledge of the intervention received, then a high risk of bias is justified. The adapted Counselor Effectiveness Scale, used by Schinke et al. ( 1978 ) required participants to rate their attitudes towards their own performance. In this study, students were aware of which intervention group they belonged to, yet the waiting list control procedure reduced potential issues such as social desirability, hence a rating of some concerns was considered appropriate. In the study reported by Greeno et al. ( 2017 ) and Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ), whose subjective measures included perceived empathy and self‐efficacy respectively, it seems probable that students were aware of the intervention group they belonged to. Given there were no differences between groups on either outcome measure, it seems unlikely that participants’ reporting of the outcome(s) was influenced by knowledge of the intervention received. The ‘some concerns’ rating was applied to both.

Hettinga ( 1978 ) reports that the researcher had no knowledge as to which treatment groups the participants were randomly assigned. However, the outcome assessors were the students who were completing two subjective measures—the Rosenberg Self‐Esteem Scale and the self‐perceived interviewing competence (SPIC) questionnaire. It is likely that the students were aware of which intervention they received. The lack of change for self‐esteem meant this outcome measure was given the ‘some concerns’ rating. However, we took a more cautious approach to students’ self‐perceived interviewing competence as the results were significant. Knowledge of the intervention could have had an impact, for example, if those students in the self‐instruction group had tried harder. There was no information to determine the likelihood that assessment of the outcome was influenced by knowledge of the intervention received, which led to a conservative judgement from the reviewers of a high risk of bias for this outcome measure.

In the study conducted by Laughlin ( 1978 ), the high risk of bias is due to known differences in the measurement of the outcome between the intervention groups. Students in the self‐reinforcement group rated their own empathic responses, whereas the supervisor rated the responses of students receiving the other experimental condition. Higgins et al. ( 2019 , p. 50) point out that, ‘outcomes should be measured or ascertained using a method that is comparable across intervention groups’, which is clearly not the case in this study.

Domain 5: Risk of bias in selection of the reported result

Bias due to selective reporting can occur when all the planned results are not completely reported. Whilst there were no unusual reporting practices identified within the randomised studies, none of them had stated their intentions in a published protocol, or additional sources of information in the public domain, making decisions about the risk of bias in selection of the reported result very difficult to ascertain. Greeno et al. ( 2017 ) and Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) report on the same study, hence these papers were compared for consistency, however, they report on different outcomes, limiting the usefulness of this approach. Email contact with Elizabeth Greeno suggests that whilst the authors had a formal plan to follow, this was not published. Consequently, verifying how reported results were selected was not possible. Due to a lack of information in all the included randomised trials, we could not make a risk of bias judgement for this domain.

Overall risk of bias

Only one included study (Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ) received a low risk of bias rating overall; one study (Schinke et al.,  1978 ) was considered to have some concerns; three studies (Hettinga,  1978 ; Laughlin,  1978 ; Wells,  1976 ) received high risk of bias ratings overall and one study (reported by Greeno et al. ( 2017 ) and Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) varied between low risk and some concerns of risk of bias depending on the outcome measure reviewed. The lack of information, evident in all of the domains is problematic and may have elevated the risk of bias for some studies and in some domains. The absence of protocols or accompanying documentation for the studies has compounded this issue. Boutron et al. ( 2021 ) state that the completeness of reporting of published articles is generally poor, and that information fundamental for assessing the risk of bias is commonly missing. Whilst reporting is seen to be improving over time, the majority of the included trials were conducted in the 1970s, and are evidently, a product of their time. Where study authors have not provided sufficient information, we have indicated that information was not reported. We also acknowledge that we adopted a conservative approach, therefore we might have judged the risk of bias harshly, potentially elevating the risk of bias either at the domain level or in the overall bias judgement for some studies. Frequent discussions supported our endeavours to be consistent.

5.3.2. Risk of bias in non‐randomised studies

As shown in Table  4 , there are clear similarities across some domains as well as some marked differences in the risk of bias ratings of the non‐randomised studies, which were judged in accordance with Robins‐I. For the overall bias ratings, the review authors either judged there to be a ‘moderate’ or ‘serious’ risk of bias in each study outcome reviewed, or in one instance, a ‘no information’ rating was issued, because assessing the risk of bias was not feasible.

Risk of bias table for non‐randomised studies based on Robins‐I.

StudyDomain 1Domain 2Domain 3Domain 4Domain 5Domain 6Domain 7Overall risk of bias
Risk of bias due to confoundingRisk of bias in the selection of participantsRisk of bias in the classification of interventionsRisk of bias in the deviation of interventionsRisk of bias due to missing outcome dataRisk of bias in measurement of the outcomeRisk of bias in selection of the reported result
Barber ( ) SERIOUSLOWLOWNo informationNo informationNo informationNo informationSERIOUS
Barber ( ) SERIOUSLOWLOWNo informationNo informationNo informationNo informationSERIOUS
Collins ( )MODERATELOWSERIOUSNo informationLOW

Analogue measure

MODERATE

No informationSERIOUS

Other measures

LOW

Keefe ( )No informationLOWLOWNo informationLOWSERIOUSNo informationSERIOUS
Ouellette ( )MODERATELOWLOWNo informationLOWLOWNo informationMODERATE
Rawlings ( )MODERATELOWLOWNo informationSERIOUS

Direct practice

LOW

No informationSERIOUS

Self‐efficacy

MODERATE

Toseland ( )MODERATELOWLOWNo informationLOWNo informationNo informationMODERATE
VanCleave ( )MODERATELOWLOWNo informationLOW

Empathic response

LOW

No information

Empathic response

MODERATE

Empathic concern

SERIOUS

Empathic concern

SERIOUS

Vinton ( )No informationLOWLOWNo information

Emotional empathy

LOW

Emotional empathy

SERIOUS

No information

Emotional empathy

SERIOUS

Expressed empathy

No information

Expressed empathy

No information

Expressed empathy

No information

Domain 1: Bias due to confounding

Sterne, Higgins, et al. ( 2016 , p. 20) suggest ‘baseline confounding is likely to be an issue in most or all NRSI’, which was reflected in the included studies of this review. The lack of information in two of the studies (Keefe,  1979 ; Vinton & Harrington,  1994 ) meant that an assessment of bias of confounding could not be provided. The other non‐randomised studies were rated as having at least moderate risks of confounding, since by the nature of their designs, causal attribution was not possible. As one study author comments, ‘selection of a nonrandom design subjected the research to confounds and threats to validity’ (VanCleave,  2007 , p. 105). Indeed, VanCleave ( 2007 ) discusses optimal group equivalency, and suggests that the distributions of some key confounders ‘fell pretty evenly’ (p. 135) between the intervention and control groups hence a moderate risk of bias was appropriate.

Whilst it is clearly not possible to control for all confounders, attempts were made by some study authors to use an analysis method that controlled for some of the most obvious ones, resulting in judgements of moderate risk of bias. Collins ( 1984 ) measured pre‐existing group differences, analysed them using a χ 2 test and found them to be unproblematic. Toseland and Spielberg ( 1982 ) used χ 2 and Kendall's T to measure a wide range of confounding variables, such as age, educational experiences and previous human services experience, from which they determined that students in the intervention and control groups were similar to one another regarding key characteristics. Ouellette et al. ( 2006 ) performed similar analyses on a wider range of confounding variables, which included age, credit hours and hours per week of paid employment undertaken during the semester, previous interviewing experience, grade point average and paid employment hours. Age was the only variable to be statistically significant; the online group were a little older than the classroom group.

In a design comparing first and final year students, Rawlings ( 2008 ) sought to establish comparability betwixt groups based on sex, ethnicity, grade point average, and age. Again, it appeared only age was significant, reflecting the fact that the final year students were further into their studies than those entering their first year. Barber ( 1988 ) employed a similar design, however both experiments were rated as having a serious risk of bias due to confounding factors. Student characteristics were not measured in either experiment, so it is impossible to be sure that the group receiving the microskills training did not differ in some way (other than the dependent variable) to the comparator student cohort.

Domain 2: Bias in selection of participants into the studies

This domain is only concerned with ‘selection into the study based on participant characteristics observed after the start of intervention… the result is at risk of selection bias if selection into the study is related to both the intervention and the outcome (Sterne, Higgins, et al.,  2016 , p. 30). There was nothing to suggest that any students were selected based on participant characteristics after the intervention had commenced in any of the studies, therefore a low risk of bias was given to all of the studies for this domain.

Domain 3: Bias in classification of interventions

All of the non‐randomised studies used population‐level interventions therefore the population is likely to be clearly defined and the collection of the information is likely to have occurred at the time of the intervention (Sterne, Higgins, et al., 2016, p. 33). As a result, the bias ratings for this domain were low in almost all of the studies. We could have issued no information ratings but decided a low rating was probably a better reflection of the non‐randomised studies in this domain. One study provides an exception to the rule. Collins ( 1984 , p. 67) stated, ‘it was not possible to establish a control group where no laboratory training took place’. This suggests the lecture‐trained and lab‐trained groups were not as distinctly different as was necessary, hence the serious risk of bias rating was applied for this domain.

Domain 4: Bias due to deviations from intended interventions

None of the studies reported on whether deviation from the intended intervention took place, hence the no information rating was issued for this domain across all of the studies.

Domain 5: Bias due to missing data

For some of the non‐randomised studies (Collins,  1984 ; Keefe,  1979 ; Ouellette et al.,  2006 ; Toseland & Spielberg,  1982 ), data sets appeared complete or almost complete. In VanCleave's ( 2007 ) study, where attrition was slightly higher, the number of missing participants was similar across the intervention group ( N  = 3) and control group ( N  = 2); reasons for drop‐out were also provided. A low bias rating was given for the missing data domain in these studies.

In Vinton and Harrington's ( 1994 ) study, a complete data set was provided for the QMEE scores, hence a low bias rating judgement was warranted, but the absence of student numbers for the Carkhuff scores meant a bias rating for this outcome measure could not be issued. An absence of information, on which to base a judgement, was also reflected in the results of Barber's ( 1988 ) experiments.

In Rawlings' ( 2008 ) study, results were reported as if all student data were present, however data were missing for some of the entering students. It is concerning that the results tables do not acknowledge the missing data. An imputational approach such as last observation carried forward or the use of group means would have enabled missing data to be dealt with, but instead the researcher has simply analysed the data available. Given that the missingness is not explained, both reviewers agreed that a serious risk of bias was justified.

Domain 6: Bias in measurements of outcomes

The timing of outcome measurements was problematic in three of the studies. A delay of approximately 3 weeks occurred in Collins' ( 1984 ) study for students completing the analogue measures, which reduced the time gap between pre‐and‐post‐test training scores. A bias rating of moderate concern was justified given this could have led to an under‐estimation of the positive gains made by students on this outcome measure.

In Keefe's ( 1979 ) study, although students were tested after their respective interventions, the interventions were of different durations hence the data collection time points varied. These are not comparable assessment methods. The meditation group was also tested three times, thus familiarity with the test may have produced the higher scores on the Affective Sensitivity Scale, rather than demonstrating a genuine improvement. Keefe ( 1979 ) states that levels of meditation attainment were blind rated (p. 36), however students in the experiential intervention group self‐assessed only, the subjectivity of which increased bias in the measurements of outcomes. These issues elevated the risk of bias in this domain to serious.

VanCleave ( 2007 ) reports, ‘the Davis self‐inventory was completed by the participant before, or following, each 8 excerpt role played situation’ (p. 118). Inconsistency surrounding the timing of when the instrument was completed led to a serious bias rating for the outcome measure of empathic concern and perspective taking. However, a low rating was given for empathic response where timing issues were not a cause for concern and independent raters were not aware of students’ intervention group status. The different ratings applied to each outcome is represented by the split ratings for this domain in Table  4 .

The same approach of splitting the outcome measures domain was taken in Rawlings' ( 2008 ) study. The direct practice outcome was judged to have a low risk of bias rating because assessors were blinded to the intervention status, whereas the self‐efficacy outcome received a moderate risk of bias rating, as the students themselves were the outcome assessors. Given the students comprised discreet cohorts, knowledge of the intervention group was not considered problematic by the reviewers. Conversely, the self‐assessment measure in Vinton and Harrington's ( 1994 ) study warranted a serious risk of bias rating. The potential for study participants to be influenced by knowledge of the intervention they received was considerable. The emotional empathy scores of the control group dropped considerably at post‐test, which could be an indication that the students had become aware that their peers were receiving beneficial interventions aimed at developing empathy, which they were not. Discussions between students were more likely in this study given they were all in the same cohort. Contamination effects could have impacted students’ self‐assessment scores.

Independent outcome assessors and appropriate blinding were used in all of the outcome measures used in Collins' ( 1984 ) study and in the video‐tape interviews in Ouellette et al.'s ( 2006 ) study, which, with the exception of the timing issues associated with Collins' ( 1984 ) analogue measure, resulted in low bias ratings for the outcomes measures in these two studies.

Key information was lacking in some studies. Notably in Barber's ( 1988 ) experiments, a judgement about the methods of outcome assessment could not be made at all due to the absence of information. Toseland and Spielberg ( 1982 ) described their judges as being independent but did not state whether or not they were aware of which intervention the student had received. For the outcome relating to empathic response, Vinton and Harrington ( 1994 ) provided no information about blinding or the independence of the outcome assessors. Potentially then, this study is also at risk of researcher allegiance bias. If, for example, the outcome assessors were part of the same institution as the instructors and the students, or of even more concern, if the assessors were the instructors, then this could pose a serious risk of bias, because potentially they have a vested interest in the findings. It was not possible to establish assessor independence, so the reviewers opted for a ‘no information’ rating for the Carkhuff scales outcome measurement in Vinton and Harrington's ( 1994 ) study.

Research suggests that if study authors play a direct role, studies are more likely to be biased in favour of the treatment intervention (Eisner,  2009 ; Maynard et al.,  2017 ; Montgomery & Belle Weisman,  2021 ). There is a distinct possibility that researchers of the included studies delivered the interventions themselves, leading to a further source of bias. VanCleave, for example, who had 19 years of teaching experience as an adjunct in the university where her research was conducted, acknowledged that ‘the researcher acted as teacher and facilitator in the intervention, which is typically not a recommended research strategy’ (VanCleave,  2007 , p. 117). The same issue is likely present in at least some of the other non‐randomised studies, although there was a lack of information from which to establish its presence or impact.

Domain 7: Bias in selection of reported results

There was no obvious bias in the reporting of results for any of the reported outcomes in the non‐randomised studies, however, there were no protocols or a priori analysis plans with which to compare the reported outcomes with the intended outcomes. Studies were not reported elsewhere hence external consistency could not be established. The ‘no information’ category was deemed most appropriate by both reviewers.

Overall risk of bias judgement

Only two studies (Ouellette et al.,  2006 ; Toseland & Spielberg,  1982 ) received an overall bias rating of moderate, reflecting a moderate rating in the confounding domain. Other studies (Barber,  1988 ; Collins,  1984 ; Keefe,  1979 ; Rawlings,  2008 ) were considered to be at serious risk of bias overall, due to receiving a serious risk of bias rating in at least one domain. For one study (Vinton & Harrington,  1994 ), the absence of information in several domains led to a ‘No information’ rating in the overall risk of bias judgement for one outcome measure but a serious risk of bias in another. Similarly, another study (VanCleave,  2007 ) also received a split rating for the overall risk of bias domain, with a moderate risk of bias for one outcome measure and a serious risk of bias for the other.

5.4. Effects of interventions

The results, as shown in Table  5 , are reported for the data that is available, relevant to answering the research question, using either the mean post‐test differences between intervention groups and control groups or the mean change score between the two groups. As outlined in Section  5.2.1 , extreme clinical heterogeneity exists between the included studies of this review, in terms of study designs, population characteristics, intervention types and features, comparators, outcomes and outcome measures. For example, in what appears to be the most promising examples of comparable situations‐empathic understanding‐the heterogeneity of the intervention is too broad to meta‐analyse data in a meaningful way. Of the four studies measuring empathic understanding (Greeno et al.,  2017 ; Keefe,  1979 ; VanCleave,  2007 ; Vinton & Harrington,  1994 ), the intervention types and characteristics, as shown in the included studies table, are vastly different. They range from 2 days of a motivational interviewing intervention consisting of live supervision with standardised clients (Greeno et al.,  2017 ), to 3 months of role‐play and 3 weeks of meditation (Keefe,  1979 ), to a multitude of components including art and music (VanCleave,  2007 ) to the use of videotapes of an unspecified amount and time period (Vinton & Harrington,  1994 ). Meta‐analysing such disparate interventions would therefore not be meaningful.

Results table of outcomes.

First author, dateOutcome measureOutcome typeEffect Size and confidence intervals
Barber,  Counselor Rating Form (non‐verbal communication only)Level 2b—Acquisition of Knowledge

Responsive interviews:

Expertness −0.82 (−1.5421 to −0.099)

Attractiveness −0.80 (−1.5223 to −0.0818)

Trustworthiness −0.82 (−1.5402 to −0.0974)

Unresponsive interviews:

Expertness −0.84 (−1.5656 to −0.1195)

Attractiveness

−1.25 (−2.0066 to −0.4916)

Trustworthiness −0.87 (−1.5897 to −0.1404)

Barber,  Counselor Rating FormLevel 2b—Acquisition of Knowledge

Responsive interviews:

Expertness −2.80 (−3.589 to −2.027)

Attractiveness −1.49 (−2.114 to −0.861)

Trustworthiness −1.45 (−2.074 to −0.828)

Unresponsive interviews:

Expertness −1.50 (−2.132 to −0.877)

Attractiveness −1.81 (−2.466 to −1.150)

Trustworthiness −1.88 (−2.5404 to −1.2102)

Collins,  Skills Acquisition MeasureLevel 2b—Acquisition of Skills

Empathy 1.21 (0.566 to 1.844)

Warmth 1.37 (0.726 to 2.023)

Genuineness 1.77 (1.090 to 2.441)

Carkhuff stemsLevel 2b—Acquisition of Skills

Empathy 0.60 (−0.069 to 1.265)

Warmth 0.78 (0.102 to 1.448)

Genuineness 1.13 (0.444 to 1.824)

AnalogueLevel 2b—Acquisition of Skills

Empathy 1.74 (1.027 to 2.455)

Warmth 1.80 (1.078 to 2.514)

Genuineness 1.88 (1.156 to 2.605)

Greeno,  Toronto Empathy Questionnaire (TEQ)Level 2a—Modification in attitudes and perceptions (Perceived empathy)−0.26 (−0.798 to 0.274)
Motivational Interviewing Treatment Integrity (MITI) questionnaireLevel 2b—Acquisition of Skills0.24 (−0.317 to 0.797)
Pecukonis,  Self‐efficacy scaleLevel 2a—Modification in attitudes and perceptions (Self‐efficacy)Insufficient data to report effect size and confidence intervals
Motivational Interviewing Treatment Integrity (MITI) questionnaireLevel 2b—Acquisition of Skills

Empathy 0.24 (−0.319 to 0.797)

MI spirit 0.12 (−0.434 to 0.680)

% MI adherent behaviours 0.34 (−0.225 to 0.896)

% Open questions 0.15 (−0.407 to 0.707)

% Complex reflections −0.25 (−0.808 to 0.308)

Reflection: Question ratio 0.04 (−0.519 to 0.594)

Hettinga,  Rosenberg Self‐Esteem Scale (RSE)Level 2a—Modification in attitudes and perceptions (Self‐esteem)

Section 1: 0.43 (−0.481 to 1.340)

Section 2: −0.86 (−2.001 to 0.2782)

Self‐Perceived Interviewing

Competence (SPIC) Questionnaire

Level 2b—Acquisition of Skills

Section 1: 1.10 (0.131 to 2.062)

Section 2: 0.64 (−0.285 to 1.561)

Keefe,  Kagan affective sensitivity scaleLevel 2a—Modification in attitudes and perceptions

Experiential training

0.02 (−0.638 to 0.671)

Experiential plus meditation

0.32 (−0.3267 to 0.9748)

Larsen, 

Index of Therapeutic Communication

Carkhuff

Level 2b—Acquisition of Skills1.51 (1.0366 to 1.9774)
Laughlin,  Carkhuff's Empathy scaleLevel 2b—Acquisition of Skills1.22 (0.4499 to 1.9894)
Enjoyment question ranked 1 to 5Level 1—Learner ReactionsEffect size and confidence intervals cannot be calculated from data available
Ouellett,  , Basic practice interviewing scaleLevel 2b—Acquisition of Skills

Total: 0.24 (−0.661 to 1.147)

Attentiveness: 0.73 (0.029 to 1.482)

Relaxed: 0.93 (0.147 to 1.710)

Satisfaction with instruction scaleLevel 1—Learner Reactions

Learning exercises well organised

−0.21 (−0.961 to 0.540)

Learning exercises sparked my interest

−0.05 (−1.224 to 0.292)

I enjoyed participating in learning exercises

−0.23 (−0.982 to 0.520)

Instructions were clear

0.46 (−2.94 to 1.223)

Rawlings,  Self‐efficacy scaleLevel 2a—Modification in attitudes and perceptions (Self‐efficacy)

Beginning 2.50 (1.5753 to 3.425)

Exploring 1.30 (0.535 to 2.060)

Contracting 2.04 (1.1898 to 2.8999)

Case Management 2.16 (1.2896 to 3.0339)

Core conditions 1.27 (0.5147 to 2.0348)

Total 2.04 (1.1881 to 2.8977)

Direct practice skillsLevel 2b—Acquisition of Skills

Beginning 1.78 (0.9627 to 2.6006)

Exploring 1.52 (0.7298 to 2.3022)

Contracting 1.69 (0.8862 to 2.5017)

Case Management 1.67 (0.8622 to 2.4708)

Core conditions 1.28 (0.5177 to 2.0385)

Total 1.85 (1.019 to 2.6741)

Schinke,  Counselor effectiveness scaleLevel 2a—Modification in attitudes and perceptions0.93 (0.0682 to 1.7903)
Videotaped interview ratingsLevel 2b—Acquisition of Skills

Eye contact 0.75 (−0.0984 to 1.594)

Smiles 0.34 (−0.4834 to 1.1647)

Nods 0.93 (0.0684 to 1.7906)

Forward trunk lean 1.36 (0.4554 to 2.2715)

Open‐ended questions 1.01 (0.1391 to 1.876)

Closed‐ended questions −0.24 (−1.0601 to 0.582)

Content summarisations 0.98 (0.1124 to 1.8436)

Affect summarisations 0.82 (−0.0317 to 1.6719)

Incongruent response −0.68 (−1.5221 to 0.1608)

Toseland,  Carkhuff Communication IndexLevel 2b—Acquisition of Skills1.40 (0.7506 to 2.0477)
Carkhuff Discrimination IndexLevel 2b—Acquisition of knowledge−1.31 (−1.9563 to −0.6694)
Counselling Skills Evaluation Part 1 (Communication)Level 2b—Acquisition of Skills1.20 (0.5588 to 1.8327)
Counselling Skills Evaluation Part 2 (Discrimination)Level 2b—Acquisition of Knowledge−0.53 (−1.1421 to 0.0799)
VanCleave,  Davis’ Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI)Level 2a—Modification in attitudes and perceptions0.22 (−0.3684 to 0.8041)
Carkhuff's Index for Communication scripts (CIC)Level 2b—Acquisition of Skills1.79 (1.0969 to 2.4799)
Vinton,  Questionnaire Measure of Emotional Empathy (QMEE)Level 2a—Modification in attitudes and perceptions0.21 (−0.4536 to 0.8751)
Carkhuff's empathy scaleLevel 2b—Acquisition of Skills0.88 (0.1823 to 1.5677)
Wells, 

A variant of the Carkhuff communication test

Carkhuff's empathy scale

Level 2b—Acquisition of Skills0.84 (−0.4499 to 2.1372)

Gagnier et al. ( 2013 ) identified twelve recommendations for investigating clinical heterogeneity in systematic reviews. In terms of the review team, one of us (PM) is a methodologist and the other (ERH) has significant relevant clinical expertise. ERH regularly discussed issues relating to population, intervention and measurement characteristics with the stakeholder group‐who included educators, students and people with lived experience. This provided a range of different perspectives, encouraging us to be reflective and reflexive in our approach, including recognising our own biases. In relation to planning and the rationale for the selection of clinical variables we hoped to consider, these were described a priori in the protocol. Other methods require statistical calculations for which we did not have sufficient data. For example, we had hoped to perform a subgroup analysis relating to the intensity of the interventions, but such data were not sufficiently available‐absent in four of them and described in non‐numerical terms (e.g., as ‘extensive’ or ‘one day’) in a further three. Gagnier et al. ( 2013 ) acknowledge the challenge posed by the incomplete reporting of data.

Given the extreme clinical heterogeneity, meta‐analysis was neither feasible nor meaningful. Instead, the findings are synthesised narratively and are organised according to a refined version of a classification of educational outcomes, developed by Kirkpatrick ( 1967 ); which is well‐known and widely used. It was refined by Kraiger et al. ( 1993 ) to distinguish between cognitive, affective and skill‐based outcomes, and adapted by Barr et al. ( 2000 ) followed by Carpenter ( 2005 ) for use in social work education. The refined classification comprises: Level 1—Learners’ Reaction, Level 2a—Modification in Attitudes and Perceptions, Level 2b—Acquisition of Knowledge and Skills, Level 3—Changes in Behaviour, Level 4a—Changes in Organisational Practice and Level 4b—Benefits to Users and Carers. Most of the studies reported more than one outcome, but none included level 4 outcomes. Therefore, the findings are synthesised according to an expanded version of levels 1 to 3—learner reactions; attitudes, perceptions, self‐efficacy; knowledge; skills and behaviours.

5.4.1. The importance of empathy

Reported in 9 of the 15 included studies (Collins,  1984 ; Greeno et al.,  2017 ; Keefe,  1979 ; Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ; Laughlin,  1978 ; Pecukonis et al.,  2016 ; Toseland & Spielberg,  1982 ; VanCleave,  2007 ; Vinton & Harrington,  1994 ; Wells,  1976 ), empathy is a common topic of interest within this review. The pivotal role of empathy in social work practice is widely acknowledged (Forrester et al.,  2008 ; Gerdes & Segal,  2009 ; Lynch et al.,  2019 ), hence the need for students to develop empathic abilities is deemed critical for preparing them for social work practice (Greeno et al.,  2017 ; Zaleski,  2016 ). As a skill which can be ‘taught, increased, refined, and mediated’ (Gerdes & Segal,  2011 , p. 143), it is hardly surprising that empathy features so frequently within the empirical literature. Truax & Carkhuff ( 1967 (p. 46) describe empathy as ‘the ability to perceive accurately and sensitively the feelings, aspirations, values, beliefs and perceptions of the client, and to communicate fully this understanding to the client’. As study authors Vinton and Harrington ( 1994 , p. 71) point out, ‘these are separate but related phenomenon’. Empathy is a multifaceted phenomenon (Lietz et al.,  2011 ), often conceptualised as empathic understanding and empathic behaviour or response. Empathic understanding consists of cognitive empathy—understanding another person's thoughts or feelings and emotional empathy—the affect invoked by another person's expression of an emotion. Empathic behaviour or response is action‐based—the communicated empathic response, including verbal and non‐verbal communication, to another person's distress (based on accurate cognitive and/or emotional empathy). There is a lack of consensus regarding how empathy should be conceptualised and measured, some of which is reflected within the included studies.

5.4.2. Level 1—Learner reaction outcomes

Learner reactions include students’ satisfaction with the training and their views about the learning experience. As stated in the protocol (Reith‐Hall & Montgomery,  2019 ), learner satisfaction alone was not sufficient to be regarded as an outcome in this review, and qualitative findings were excluded. Two of the included studies gathered quantitative data on learner reactions, in addition to other outcomes. Laughlin ( 1978 ) found self‐instruction students exhibited significantly higher mean scores for enjoyment and number of optional practice items completed than students in an instructor‐led group. Laughlin ( 1978 , p. 67) suggests self‐instruction ‘creates a sense of self‐reliance, confidence, and personal responsibility for learning which promotes enjoyment and devotion to task not present under circumstances of external control’. However, there was no significant correlation between the variables of enjoyment and commitment with students’ gain scores.

Ouellette et al. ( 2006 ) issued a semester survey questionnaire, including a four‐item subscale which measured students’ perception of their satisfaction with the instruction they received—traditional classroom based versus online. Most students agreed or strongly agreed that learning exercises were clear and effective, irrespective of the type of instruction they received. There were no significant differences in their satisfaction scores. Again, there was no statistically significant correlation between students’ perceived satisfaction, perceived acquisition of interviewing skills and the independent ratings of students’ acquisition of interviewing skills, in either group.

5.4.3. Level 2a—Modification in attitudes and perceptions

Carpenter ( 2005 ,  2011 ) suggests that Level 2a outcomes relate to changes in attitudes or perceptions towards service users and carers/care‐givers, their problems and needs, circumstances, care and treatment. Motivational outcomes and self‐efficacy also comprise this level (Kraiger et al.,  1993 ).

Attitudes and perceptions towards clients

Students’ perceptions towards clients was an outcome of interest for a number of studies included in this review. Affective sensitivity (Keefe,  1979 ), emotional empathy (Vinton & Harrington,  1994 ), empathic concern and perspective taking (VanCleave,  2007 ) and perceived empathy (Greeno et al.,  2017 ) all fit under the umbrella term of empathic understanding. Within the literature, empathic understanding has been further defined as an affective process and a cognitive process. These different ways of conceptualising empathy are evident within the included studies, and in the choice of measuring instruments the researchers employed.

Affective and cognitive outcomes

To ascertain students’ abilities to detect and describe the immediate affective state of clients, Keefe ( 1979 ) employed Kagan's scale of affective sensitivity (Campbell et al.,  1971 ), which consists of multiple‐choice items used with a series of short, videotaped excerpts from actual counselling sessions. In Keefe's study, a positive and significant effect size of 0.32 was only found once the intervention group had been taught meditation in addition to the experiential training they received, correlating with blind ranked levels of meditation attainment. Keefe ( 1979 ) reported that the combined effects of both conditions produced mean empathy levels beyond those attained by master's and doctoral students. Segal et al. ( 2017 , p. 98) suggest that using meditation can promote emotional regulation, which can be considered fundamental to empathy. Dupper ( 2017 , p. 31) suggests that mindfulness is an effective strategy for ‘reducing implicit bias and fostering empathy towards members of stigmatised outgroups’. Both propositions could explain why the combined interventions in Keefe's ( 1979 ) study proved most effective.

Also viewing empathy as an affective state, Vinton and Harrington ( 1994 ) sought to assess students’ ‘emotional empathy’, which they describe as ‘the ability to be affected by the client's emotional state’ (p. 71). Vinton and Harrington ( 1994 ) employed a different outcome measure—the Questionnaire Measure of Emotional Empathy (QMEE) (Mehrabian & Epstein,  1972 ), which emphasises the affective component of empathy including emotional arousal to others’ distress. Two intervention groups received an instruction package utilising videotapes, one relying on self‐instruction, the other also receiving input from an instructor and peer group, whilst the control group received no intervention. At post‐test, we found a small effect size of 0.21 between the ‘video other and self’ and the controls, however the QMEE scores of both groups had actually declined. Despite these results, Vinton and Harrington ( 1994 ) suggested that further investigation into the use of videotape or film is warranted.

Building on the suggestion by Vinton and Harrington ( 1994 ) that film can assist the development of empathic understanding, the students in VanCleave's ( 2007 ) study watched a 2‐h commercial film, with 30 min of reflection and discussion. The self‐report measure they used comprised two subscales from the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) (Davis,  1980 ): the first, empathic concern addresses the affective component of empathy and the second, perspective taking focusses on the cognitive component of empathy. Despite using a broader conceptualisation of empathy and a more inclusive measure, which produced an effect size of 0.22, changes were not statistically significant.

Utilising a different instrument still, Greeno et al. ( 2017 ) sought to measure students’ perceived empathy using the Toronto Empathy Questionnaire (TEQ) (Spreng et al.,  2009 ), which views empathy as an emotional process, but is based on items from the QMEE and the IRI. The effect size at post‐test was −0.26, with study authors reporting no statistically significant difference between groups. Given a behavioural measure of empathy used by Greeno et al. ( 2017 ) demonstrated a statistically significant small effect size for the intervention group, ‘the lack of change across time and groups’ on the self‐reported TEQ scores was ‘unexpected’ (p. 803).

No statistically significant changes in students’ empathic understanding were identified in the studies above, irrespective of the type of self‐report measure used. The challenges of measuring empathy through self‐reports (Lietz et al.,  2011 ) are clearly evident in this review and will be discussed further in Section 6.

Perceptions of the treatment/intervention

Based on the same study reported by Greeno et al. ( 2017 ), Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) issued a 17‐item self‐report measure to garner students’ perceptions of Motivational Interviewing. Training for the intervention group included real‐time feedback by clinical supervisors whereas the control group received online TAU. No between group difference was identified, however perceptions of the Motivational Interviewing increased (by an average of 7 points) for both groups over time.

Self‐esteem and self‐efficacy

Self‐esteem, which reflects how people perceive themselves and includes a sense of goodness or worthiness, was an outcome measure in just one of the included studies. Hettinga ( 1978 ) argued that self‐esteem, as a critical dimension of professional self‐dependence, directly relates to the attainment of skills. However, he used The Rosenberg Self‐Esteem Scale (RSE) (1965), an instrument measuring global self‐esteem, in his study. For students in the intervention group, who experienced videotaped interview playback with instructional feedback, the self‐esteem score dropped very slightly. For the control condition, who received feedback delivered in a small group format, the self‐esteem score remained unchanged. Although we found a small effect size for Section  1 , Hettinga suggested the findings were not significant, indicating the intervention had no impact on students’ self‐esteem scores.

Parker ( 2006 ) differentiates between the global nature of self‐esteem and the context specific nature of self‐efficacy. Perceived self‐efficacy beliefs ‘influence whether people think pessimistically or optimistically and in ways that are self‐enhancing or self‐hindering’ (Bandura,  2001 , p. 10), which has implications for students’ skill development. Self‐efficacy is ‘an individual's assessment of his or her confidence in their ability to execute specific skills in a particular set of circumstances and thereby achieve a successful outcome’ (Bandura,  1986 , as quoted in Holden et al.,  2002 ). Literature in the counselling field indicates that self‐efficacy may predict performance (Larson & Daniels,  1998 ), and can thus serve as a proxy measure. The idea that self‐efficacy is a means to assess outcomes in social work education has gained traction in recent years (Holden et al.,  2002 ,  2017 ; Quinney & Parker,  2010 ).

Two of the included studies measured self‐efficacy. Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) found no change in students’ self‐efficacy scores, either between the brief motivational interviewing intervention group and the TAU control group, or over time. Rawlings ( 2008 ), who evaluated the impact of an entire university degree, found students exiting Bachelor of Social Work (BSW) Education had significantly higher self‐efficacy scores (mean score of 6.78) than those entering it (mean score of 4.40). Through multiple regression analysis, results showed that BSW education positively predicted self‐efficacy. However, students’ self‐efficacy ratings did not correlate with their practice skill ratings. Surprisingly, after controlling for BSW education, self‐efficacy was found to be a negative predictor of direct practice skill. Rawlings ( 2008 , p. xi) explains that ‘self‐efficacy acted as a suppressor variable in mediating the relationship between education and skill’. This unexpected finding reflects the controversy surrounding the use of self‐efficacy as an outcome measure, which will be revisited in Section  6.3 .

Schinke et al. ( 1978 ) asked students to rate their attitudes towards their own role‐played interviewing performance. A large effect size of 0.93 indicates that CST positively affected the attitudes students had about their performance.

5.4.4. Level 2b—Acquisition of knowledge and skills

The acquisition of knowledge relates to the concepts, procedures and principles of working with service users and carers. Carpenter ( 2005 ), after Kraiger et al. ( 1993 ), separated knowledge outcomes into declarative knowledge, procedural knowledge and strategic knowledge. Only procedural knowledge—‘that used in the performance of a task’ (Carpenter,  2011 , p. 126), featured as an outcome in this review, reported in three studies (two publications).

Procedural knowledge

Barber,  1988 (p. 4) anticipated that students beginning their training would have ‘little knowledge of correct interviewing behaviour’. Conversely, he expected students approaching the end of their training to be more able to judge responsive and unresponsive non‐verbal communication—displayed by actors towards simulated clients (in experiment 1) and practitioners towards real clients (in experiment 2). The anticipated enhanced judgement that microskills training was expected to elicit can be identified as what Kraiger et al. ( 1993 ) referred to as procedural knowledge. The experiments used case studies to which students were asked to respond; Carpenter ( 2011 ) suggests these are appropriate measures to assess procedural knowledge in social work education.

Contrary to his expectations, and the findings of the other studies in this review, the two experiments conducted by Barber ( 1988 ) found that the reactions of students who had received microskills training were less accurate than the reactions of untrained students. In the first experiment, the untrained comparator group rated counsellor responsiveness higher than the trained intervention group, with large effect sizes between the groups for expertness (−0.82), attractiveness (−0.80), and trustworthiness (−0.82). The same pattern emerged when rating counsellor unresponsiveness, with large effect sizes for expertness (−0.84), attractiveness (−1.25) and trustworthiness (−0.87). Flaws in the first experiment include that video segments assessed by students were just 2 min long and included non‐verbal communication only, which goes some way towards explaining the surprising results. Whilst non‐verbal communication is extremely important, the absence of the verbal accompaniment and speech tone, emphasis and pacing, does not reflect how most people communicate, either in their personal lives or in social work practice, nor does it provide students with an opportunity to identify mirroring or mimicry. Barber ( 1988 ) acknowledges that artificiality might have led to trained students being more critical than their non‐trained counterparts.

In the second of Barber's experiments, the untrained comparator group rated counsellor responsiveness higher than the trained intervention group, with very large effect sizes between the groups for expertness (−2.80), attractiveness (−1.49) and trustworthiness (−1.45). A similar trend occurred when rating counsellor unresponsiveness with large effect sizes for expertness (−1.50), attractiveness (−1.81) and trustworthiness (−1.88). Barber ( 1988 ) found untrained students performed similarly to clients’ ratings, which he perceived as evidence that the trained students were underperforming. However, it is possible that the trained students were looking out for different responses than the untrained students and clients. Barber speculated that training reduced student's capacity to empathise with the client, however, the outcomes of interest—trustworthiness, attractiveness and expertness, which is what students were asked to rate, do not measure empathy, hence the face validity of this measurement is questionable. After completing a factor analysis of a shortened version of the Counsellor Rating Form used in Barber's experiments, Tryon ( 1987 , p. 126) concluded that ‘further information about what it measures, and how, is needed’. It is hard to fathom how the conclusions Barber drew, were born out of the measures he employed and the results these measures produced.

Design limitations are also apparent, with Barber acknowledging that the first year and final year student groups may have been different to each other on variables other than the training. The experiments are important, because the findings that social work students appeared less able to judge responsive and unresponsive interviewing behaviour after training in microskills than counterparts who had yet to receive the training would suggest this teaching intervention could have an adverse, undesirable or harmful effect. However, other studies which ensured that students were matched on factors such as demographic variables and pre‐course experience (e.g., Toseland & Spielberg,  1982 ), produced more positive results. Thus, Barber's paper is an exception to the rule, such that his findings should be interpreted cautiously, with due consideration of the measurement and design issues evident within both experiments and the serious risk of bias, due to confounding.

In Toseland and Spielberg's ( 1982 ) study, two of the four measures employed also tap into the procedural knowledge outcome because students judged the ability of others to respond in a helpful way. First, a film of client vignettes was shown to students who had to select from five different responses, rating them from ‘destructive’ to ‘most helpful’ using the second part of a Counselling Skills Evaluation. Second, through the Carkhuff's Discrimination Index (Carkhuff,  1969a ), students rated the helpfulness of four counsellor responses to a set of client statements. Difference scores were generated by comparing students’ ratings with those produced by trained judges. Discrimination scores indicated that students who had received the training were better able to discriminate between effective and ineffective responses to clients’ problems, and their ratings closely matched those of trained judges. With effect sizes of −1.31 for the Carkhuff Discrimination Index and −0.53 for the Counselling Skills Evaluation part 2, and a very high confidence level of 0.001, the findings were significant.

Skills have been organised hierarchically within the literature on social work education outcomes to include initial skill acquisition, skill compilation and skill automaticity (Carpenter,  2005 ,  2011 ; Kraiger et al.,  1993 ). Skill automaticity did not feature as an outcome in this review, which possibly reflects the point made by Carpenter ( 2005 ); that ‘the measurement of the highest level of skill development, automaticity, poses significant problems’ (p. 14). To our knowledge, no valid measure of automaticity for communication skills currently exists.

Initial skills

Initial skills, which are often practised individually, in response to short statements or vignettes, were the most popular outcome reported in this review. ‘Trainee behaviour at the initial skill acquisition stage of development may be characterised as rudimentary in nature’ (Kraiger et al.,  1993 , p. 316).

The initial skills considered fundamental for demonstrating empathy were evidently interesting to the researchers of the included studies. Variations of the Carkhuff scales (Carkhuff,  1969a ,  1969b ), which are widely used in social work education (Hepworth et al.,  2010 ), were employed in seven of the included studies (Collins,  1984 ; Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ; Laughlin,  1978 ; Toseland & Spielberg,  1982 ; VanCleave,  2007 ; Vinton & Harrington,  1994 ; Wells,  1976 ). The Carkhuff scales comprise two subsets: empathy discrimination (being able to accurately identify the level of empathy response) and empathy communication (putting that discriminated empathy into a congruent action response) (Carkhuff,  1969a ,  1969b ). The Carkhuff scales can require either a written or verbal response to a written statement or audio/video vignette, although instruction was originally mediated through audio recordings (Toukmanian & Rennie,  1975 ). Independent raters evaluate the level of empathy shown, selecting from five levels whereby level one represents low levels of empathy and level five indicates high levels. Level three is considered to be a minimally facilitative empathic response.

Using a slightly adapted version of the written statements format of the Carkhuff ( 1969b ) scale, Larsen and Hepworth ( 1978 ) assessed students’ skill levels in providing empathic responses to ‘written messages’, which they suggest was highly significant ( p  < 0.001). We calculated a large effect size (1.51), demonstrating as predicted, that the experimental groups surpassed the control groups on achieved levels of performance.

Toseland and Spielberg ( 1982 ) sought to replicate and expand on Larsen and Hepworth's ( 1978 ) study by developing and evaluating a training programme comprising core helping skills, including genuineness, warmth and empathy. Two of the measures they used capture the initial skills outcome. First, through Carkhuff's Communication Index, as described above, students were asked to act as though they were the worker and respond by writing what they would say to a set of statements. Second, through part 1 of a Counselling Skills Evaluation (CSE), students watched a film of client vignettes, and wrote what they would say if they were the worker. Student responses to both measures were rated by trained judges. Students in the control group saw a slight reduction in their skills on both measures whereas the intervention group demonstrated gains on both measures with large effect sizes of 1.40 on the Carkhuff Communication Index and 1.20 on part 1 of the Counselling Skills Evaluation. Students in receipt of the training increased their ability to communicate effectively using the ten helping skills.

Nerdrum and Lundquist ( 1995 ) suggest that because Larsen and Hepworth ( 1978 ) and Toseland and Spielberg ( 1982 ) reported ratings for total communication index rather than empathy specifically, that lower empathy scores may have been concealed. Certainly, the instructors in the study reported by Nerdrum and colleagues (Nerdrum,  1997 ; Nerdrum & Høglend,  2003 ; Nerdrum & Lundquist,  1995 ), which narrowly missed the inclusion criteria for this review, found that empathy was the most difficult of the facilitative conditions for students to grasp. In addition, methods of training and methods of measurement have been confounded in earlier studies, potentially leading to over inflated treatment effects (Nerdrum & Høglend,  2003 ).

To evaluate an interviewing skills course, Laughlin ( 1978 ), also using the Carkhuff instrument, sought to test self‐instructional methods, in which one experimental condition relied on self‐reinforcement whilst the other experimental condition received external reinforcement and feedback from an instructor. Both experimental groups produced greater learning gains after training than either of the two control groups. Interestingly, there was no significant difference between the gain scores of the two experimental groups. Laughlin ( 1978 , p. 65) suggests that ‘self‐managed behavior change can, under certain circumstances, prove to be as efficacious as externally controlled systems of behavior change’. However, students in the self‐reinforcement group rated their own empathic responses, whereas the supervisor rated the responses of students receiving the other experimental condition. As Laughlin ( 1978 ) acknowledged, ‘the self‐instruction group may be considered a product of inaccuracy in the self‐evaluation process’ (p. 68). Other studies have identified that students often over or underestimate their abilities (Kruger & Dunning,  1999 ). Based on their mean gain scores, we calculated a large effect size of 1.22 between the experimental condition who received external reinforcement and feedback and the control group who received no instruction.

Vinton and Harrington ( 1994 ) also appear interested in the role of the self in student learning, and they too used the Carkhuff scales to investigate this issue. At post‐test, a large effect size (0.88) was observed between the ‘videotape self and other’ group and the controls. At one month follow‐up, Vinton and Harrington ( 1994 ) found the majority of students in the intervention groups reached the level Carkhuff deemed to be facilitative.

To compare the effects of roleplay and using participants’ own problems for developing empathic communication skills through facilitative training, Wells ( 1976 ) used a variant of Carkhuff ( 1969a ) communication test in which students were asked to respond empathically in writing to four tape‐recorded helpee statements before training and to a different set of four statements after training. Contrary to Wells’ assertion that no differential effect between role‐play and ‘own problems’ procedures was identified and the suggestion that active experimentation of students in both groups explains their modest outcome gains, we found a large effect size of 0.84 at post‐test. This finding should be interpreted cautiously given it is based on just five students per group.

Collins ( 1984 ) used two written skills measures—the Carkhuff stems, using written client statements as stimuli and a Skills Assessment Measure (SAM), which uses an audio‐video client stimulus. Both measures seek to capture outcomes that can be categorised as initial skills. The mean scores on the Carkhuff stems at post‐test were slightly higher for lab trained students than lecture trained students. Effect sizes were 0.60, 0.78 and 1.13 for empathy, warmth and genuineness respectively. However, Collins ( 1984 ) reports that statistical significance was only reached for empathy, which he suggests might be because lecture and lab training prepare students for training on the relatively straightforward measure of producing written statements as responses to short client vignettes. Warmth and genuineness might be easier to demonstrate than empathy hence lecture‐based students could manage them satisfactorily.

Similar, but slightly higher findings were demonstrated through the Skills Acquisition Measure (SAM), wherein students were asked to respond in writing to a series of vignettes. They were advised that their responses should be based on what they would say if they were conducting the interview. Student responses to the SAM were scored by trained raters using the Carkhuff scales. The post‐test scores of lab‐trained students compared favourably with the lecture‐trained students. Large effect sizes of 1.21, 1.37 and 1.77 were found empathy, warmth and genuineness respectively. Collins ( 1984 ) concluded that findings from the Carkhuff stems and the Skills Acquisition Measure provide evidence that lab‐based training is more effective for teaching interpersonal interviewing skills for social work students than lecture‐based training.

Carkhuff ( 1969a ) suggested similarities between responses to the stimulus expressions in written form and verbal form and responses offered in an actual interview with a client. However, it should be noted that this alleged equivalency of measures has been questioned throughout the literature. VanCleave ( 2007 ) noted that making an advanced verbal empathic response is arguably more challenging than producing written statements. In her study, expert raters used the Carkhuff's Index for Communication scripts (CIC) to evaluate the videotaped responses of students to actors who verbally delivered excerpts based on the Carkhuff stems. Tapes contained vignette responses, rather than role‐played sessions in their entirety. With a large effect size of 1.79, students in the intervention group demonstrated more empathy than the students who did not receive the empathy response training.

In summary, multiple studies demonstrated an increase in social work students’ communication skills, including empathy, following training. The results for actual skill demonstration are modest yet promising.

Compilation

The compilation of skills is the term coined by Kraiger et al. ( 1993 ) to refer ‘to the grouping of skills into fluid behaviour’ (Carpenter,  2005 , p. 12). Methods for measuring the compilation of skills include students’ self‐rating of competencies and observer ratings of students’ communication skills in simulated interviews (Carpenter,  2011 ). Wilt ( 2012 ) argued that simulation fosters more in‐depth learning than discussions, case studies, and role‐plays, due to the location of the student in the role of the worker and real‐time decision‐making that includes ethical considerations.

In the study by Collins ( 1984 ), analogue interviews, which consisted of a 10‐min role‐play of a student in the worker role with a student in the client role, showed modest gains, whereby 23% of students in the lab group improved by 0.5, to a level which Carkhuff and Berenson ( 1976 ) suggested was the sign of an effective intervention. This was significantly lower than the 52% who showed 0.5 improvement on the Skills Acquisition Measure. However, Collins ( 1984 ) suggests that direct comparisons of the findings is problematic given the delay (of approximately 3 weeks) in students completing the analogue measures, which reduced the time gap between pre‐and‐post‐training scores. Despite this, improvements shown in the analogue interviews were still significant. When comparing the two interventions—lab versus lecture, the lab‐trained students demonstrated more skill than the lecture‐trained group, as demonstrated by very large effect sizes of 1.74 for empathy, 1.80 for warmth and 1.88 for genuineness.

Hettinga ( 1978 ) sought to measure the impact of videotaped interview playback with instructional feedback on student social workers interviewing skills. A tailor‐made instrument was used to measure self‐perceived interviewing competence (SPIC). At post‐test, the mean score for the combined intervention groups was 62.60 whereas for the control groups the mean score was 57.47. This finding was supported by moderate to large effect sizes of 1.10 for Section  1 and 0.64 for Section  2 , albeit with small sample sizes. The significantly higher scores for the intervention group suggest that students’ self‐perceived interviewing competence was positively impacted by videotaped interview playback with instructional feedback. Hettinga ( 1978 ) acknowledged the problem of using self‐reports as a measure of skill accomplishment. This is considered further in Section  6.3 .

Both methods (self‐ratings and observer ratings) were used in the study conducted by Schinke et al. ( 1978 ). Through 10‐min videotaped role‐play simulations at pre‐ and post‐test, expert raters assessed a range of verbal and non‐verbal communication skills demonstrated by students. The largest effect sizes were for forward trunk lean (1.36) and open‐ended questions (1.01). After completing the videoed role‐plays, students rated their own interviewing skills according to an adapted version of the Counselor Effectiveness Scale developed by Ivey and Authier ( 1971 ). The intervention group's mean change score of 37.083 was significantly higher than the control group's mean change score of 13.182, producing an effect size of 0.93.

Ouellette et al. ( 2006 ) employed similar methods‐a 10‐min videotaped role‐play simulation and student self‐rating scale‐to evaluate the actual acquisition of interviewing skills between students taught in a traditional face to face class and students using a Web‐based instructional format with no face‐to‐face contact with the instructor. Rated according to a Basic Practice Interviewing Skills scale, very few statistically significant differences were found between the traditional class and the online class. Significant differences were identified for only 2 of 21 specific interviewing skills ratings, with an effect size of 0.73 for attentiveness and 0.93 for being relaxed. The findings indicate that for two of the interviewing skills measured, the online students were slightly more proficient than their peers in the traditional class. In a semester survey questionnaire, including a four‐item subscale measuring students’ perception of their acquisition of beginning interviewing skills, Ouellette et al. ( 2006 ) found few statistical differences between the groups apart from the classroom group responded more favourably in terms of their perception of learning a lot from the pedagogical activities used to teach interviewing skills. The interviewing skills of an online class versus those taught in a traditional face‐to‐face classroom setting were ‘approximately equal’ on completion of an interviewing skills course (Ouellette et al.,  2006 , p. 68).

In the study reported by Greeno et al. ( 2017 ) and Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ), which investigated motivational interviewing, students’ empathic skills were observed and rated from low (score of 1) to high (score of 5) using the Motivational Interviewing Treatment Integrity (MITI) questionnaire. This measure, specific to the treatment modality of the intervention, provides a global empathy score, which aims to capture all of the efforts the student/practitioner makes to understand the client's perspective and convey this understanding to the client. Greeno et al. ( 2017 ) found improvements were evident for the intervention group, who received live supervision with simulated clients. At post‐test, the authors observed a small effect size of 0.24. The intervention group maintained gains at follow up, hence Greeno et al. ( 2017 ) conclude, ‘results from the study cautiously lend evidence that suggests live supervision as a promising practice for teaching MI to social work students’ (p. 803). These findings are particularly important given this is one of only two outcomes across all of the included studies to receive a low risk of bias rating.

Referring to the same study, Pecukonis et al.'s ( 2016 ) trained MITI coders produced summary scores deriving from the following behaviour counts. They found that the change scores between the start of the intervention and follow‐up were 1.39 for the live supervision group and −0.85 for the TAU group, providing support that Live Supervision was effective in teaching the early stages of MI skills. For empathy, at post‐test, a small effect size of 0.24 was observed. For the percentage of Motivational Interviewing adherent behaviours, an effect size of 0.34 was identified. Differences were less pronounced for MI specific skills. The authors observed that the intervention group displayed trends of attaining higher levels of proficiency on MI specific skills compared with the TAU group. An exception to this trend was observed at post‐test for percentage of complex reflections,—effect size −0.25, although they had lost this gain by follow‐up. Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) identify that statistical significance was seen only for the MI area of reflection to question ratio, acknowledging that the study may be underpowered.

Rawlings ( 2008 ) compared the performance of direct practice skills of students entering an undergraduate social work course with students exiting the same course. Students completed a 15‐min video‐taped interview with a standardised client. Students’ performance was evaluated by independent raters using an adapted version of a 14‐item instrument, developed by Chang and Scott ( 1999 ) to rate basic practice skills including beginning, exploring, contracting, case management skills, and the core conditions of genuineness, warmth, and empathy. Exiting students scored higher than entering students on each practice skill set, with a large effect size of 1.85 for the overall total score.

Studies measuring the compilation of skills demonstrated modest gains in students’ communicative abilities, including general social work interviewing skills and the demonstration of expressed empathy.

5.4.5. Level 3: Behaviour and the implementation of learning into practice

Collins ( 1984 ) was the only study in this review to include a behavioural outcome. Scores from client interviews, which consisted of tape‐recorded interviews with clients at the start of their field practicums, were compared to scores from the analogue role‐play interviews at the end of the training to investigate the transfer of skills into practice. There was a drop for lab‐trained students from their analogue role‐play scores to their client interviews—from 2.72 to 2.22 ( T  = 7.59) for empathy, 2.79 to 2.35 ( T  = 6.82) for warmth and 2.63 to 2.28 ( T  = 6.65) for genuineness. These findings suggest students did not transfer their learning from the laboratory into practice, which Collins ( 1984 ) suggests was because of measurement anxiety, problems with the measures and the fundamental differences between lab and fieldwork settings.

5.4.6. Level 4a: Changes in organisational practice

None of the included studies addressed this outcome.

5.4.7. Level 4b: Benefits to users and carers

6. discussion, 6.1. summary of main results.

The purpose of this systematic review was to identify, summarise, evaluate and synthesise the current body of evidence to establish if CST programmes for social work students are effective. Fifteen studies were included in this review. Most of the studies included in this review are dated, methodological rigour was weak, quality was poor, and the risk of bias was moderate to high/serious or had to be rated as incomplete due to limitations in reporting. Extreme heterogeneity exists between the primary studies and the interventions they evaluated, precluding the meaningful synthesis of effect sizes through meta‐analysis. The findings of this review are therefore limited and must be interpreted with caution.

The anticipated outcome of a positive change in the modification of perceptions and attitudes of students (including cognitive and affective changes) following training was not born out in the data. This may in part be a result of how these outcomes are conceptualised and measured, with self‐reports being particularly problematic. Of the 15 included studies in this review, two studies, reported in one paper (Barber,  1988 ) ( N  = 82) identified a negative outcome for the acquisition of knowledge, whereby trained students placed less value on responsive and unresponsive interviewing behaviour and were less accurate in their ability to predict clients’ reactions than their untrained counterparts. However, there was no convincing evidence to suggest that the teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education causes adverse or harmful effects.

For the outcome of skills acquisition, which featured in 12 of the included studies, reported in thirteen papers, only one study (Ouellette et al.,  2006 ) ( N  = 30), which compared face‐to‐face and online instruction, did not find a significant difference between the groups. Effect sizes in the other 11 studies measuring skills acquisition (Collins,  1984 ; Greeno et al.,  2017 ; Hettinga,  1978 ; Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ; Laughlin,  1978 ; Pecukonis et al.,  2016 ; Rawlings,  2008 ; Schinke et al.,  1978 ; Toseland & Spielberg,  1982 ; VanCleave,  2007 ; Vinton & Harrington,  1994 ; Wells,  1976 ) ( N  = 575) indicated some identifiable improvements in the communication skills including empathy, in students who received training. This finding is in keeping with reviews about CST (Aspegren,  1999 ) and empathy training (Batt‐Rawden et al.,  2013 ) for medical students and nursing students (Brunero et al.,  2010 ).

The review identified considerable gaps within the evidence, further research is required. This is discussed in Section  7 .

6.1.1. Level 1: Learner reactions

The evidence was inconclusive as only two studies ( N  = 108) contributed data. However, the findings, whilst limited, reflect a criticism of the growing trend, in the UK at least, to rely on quality assurance templates, which collect end of course satisfaction ratings only, and fail to measure outcomes (Carpenter,  2011 ).

6.1.2. Level 2a: Modification in attitudes and perceptions

One study ( N  = 23), Schinke et al. ( 1978 ) found that students’ positive attitudes towards their skills were almost three times higher among students who had received CST than those who had not. Whilst promising, the evidence was inconclusive because too few studies contributed data. The review also highlights the challenges of using self‐reports to measure empathic understanding; no statistically significant changes were identified in three of four studies investigating empathic understanding, despite the same studies demonstrating the positive gains established when utilising other outcome measures. The challenges of measuring empathy through self‐reports (Lietz et al.,  2011 ) are well documented and discussed further in Section  6.3 .

6.1.3. Level 2b: Modification in knowledge

The evidence was inconclusive, because only three studies (reported in two publications) ( N  = 150) contributed data. In a review of empathy training evaluation research, Lam et al.,  2011 found that regardless of the training method used, individuals were able to learn about the concept of empathy. Whilst the modification of knowledge is relatively straightforward, this was evidently not an outcome reported in the studies in this review.

6.1.4. Level 2b: Modification of skills

The evidence does suggest that modest gains can be made in the interviewing skills and the demonstration of empathic abilities of student social workers following systematic CST. This was the strongest finding of this review with 12 out of the 15 studies ( N  = 605) contributing data, 11 of which reported improvements for students in the intervention groups.

6.1.5. Level 3: Changes in behaviour

The evidence was inconclusive due to the fact only one study ( N  = 67) reported this outcome.

6.1.6. Level 4: Changes in organisational practice and benefits to users and carers

The outcomes was not addressed in any of the studies included in this review.

6.1.7. Adverse effects

The evidence was inconclusive as only one paper ( N  = 82) contributed data.

6.2. Overall completeness and applicability of evidence

The included studies indicate, albeit tentatively, that interventions for teaching communication skills in social work education seem to have a positive impact, at least on demonstrable skills outcomes, and in the short‐term. Only Barber ( 1988 ) based on his own empirical research, questioned whether microskills were worth teaching. Perhaps the starkest finding of the review is the paucity of high quality and rigorously designed studies intended to present evidence for the outcomes of teaching communication skills to social work students, particularly given that pedagogic practices in the teaching and learning of communication skills are well established in social work education across the globe. Many of the included studies are quite dated and the majority were conducted in the United States. The picture provided by the existing body of evidence is incomplete‐it does not reflect the involvement of people with lived experience, or the newer innovations or technological advances used in social work education today‐limiting the applicability of the evidence.

In terms of publication bias, we recognise that there will be some PhD theses and trials containing negative results which we have not located in this review, and we acknowledge that publication bias could potentially be an issue. We took steps to minimise the risks including a wide reaching and extensive search (excluding outcomes) and contacting subject experts to identify any publications we might have missed through our search strategy. Strategies typically used to assess publication bias, such as funnel plots, were not feasible due to their small size and number, and lack of power.

Extreme levels of heterogeneity and moderate to high/serious risk of bias ratings in the studies included in the review, meant meta‐analysis was not feasible, and consequently a narrative review was undertaken. Outcomes were analysed and structured according to the outcomes framework for social work education developed by Carpenter ( 2005 ), after Kirkpatrick ( 1967 ), Kraiger et al. ( 1993 ) and Barr et al. ( 2000 ). Although data exists for some outcomes in levels 1–3, none of the included studies addressed outcomes at level 4a—changes in organisational practice or level 4b—benefits to users and carers, therefore significant gaps in the evidence base remain.

6.3. Quality of the evidence

Whilst there was overall consistency in the direction of mean change for the development of communication skills of social work students following training, we must acknowledge that the body of evidence is small in terms of eligible studies and that rigour across this body of evidence is low. The assessment of methodological quality and the risk of bias, examined using the ROB 2 tool for randomised trials and the ROBINS‐I tool for non‐randomised study, was judged to be moderate to high/serious, or incomplete, in all but one of the included studies. Confounders such as differences at baseline, missing data and the failure to address missingness appropriately, and the knowledge outcome assessors had about the intervention and its recipients were the most significant detractors from the internal validity of the studies reviewed.

Empathy has featured in skills training for more than 50 years, however as the studies in this review indicate, ‘evidence of empathy training in the social work curriculum, remains scarce and sketchy’ (Gerdes & Segal,  2011 , p. 142). As Gair ( 2011 , p. 791) maintains, ‘comprehensive discussion about how to specifically cultivate, teach and learn empathy is not common in the social work literature’, and the evidence that does exist is fairly limited. The same criticisms have been levied against research into the teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education more generally (Dinham,  2006 ; Trevithick et al.,  2004 ). Given the range and extent of bias identified within this body of evidence, caution should be exercised in judging the efficacy of the interventions for improving the communicative abilities of social work students.

6.3.1. Concerns about definitions and conceptualisations

One of the challenges evident in this review is the considerable variation in the way the study authors define key constructs, particularly in relation to empathy. Defining empathy remains problematic (Batt‐Rawden et al.,  2013 ) because the construct of empathy lacks clarity and consensus (Gerdes et al.,  2010 ) and conceptualisations have changed over time. Whilst cognitive, neurobiological, behavioural, and emotional components are now recognised (Lietz et al.,  2011 ), earlier conceptualisations were more unidimensional, depicting empathy as a trait, emotion or skill. As a result, there is no consistency in the way operational definitions of empathy are used between the studies in this review, which has further implications for how outcomes are measured and restricts what the body of evidence can confidently tell us. The issue is not unique to social work; referring to a health context, Robieux et al.,  2018 , p. 59) suggest that ‘research faces a challenge to find a shared, adequate and scientific definition of empathy’.

6.3.2. Concerns about measures

Communication skills, including empathy, can be measured from different perspectives including self‐rating (first person assessment), service user/patient‐rating (second person assessment) and observer rating (third person assessment) (Hemmerdinger et al.,  2007 ). Ratings from service users were absent from the included studies, possibly because of geographical factors. Most of the included studies were conducted in North America where the inclusion of service users and carers in social work education is less prominent than in the UK, for example. Many of the included studies used validated scales whereas others developed their own measures. However, even with validated scales, measurement problems were encountered by the study authors.

Self‐rating

Much of the outcome data in social work education has relied on self‐report, a trend reflected in this review. Self‐reports appeared appropriate for measuring satisfaction with teaching and practice interventions in Laughlin ( 1978 ) and Ouellette et al.'s ( 2006 ) studies, although these outcomes did not correlate to student's improvement in skills. Self‐efficacy scales are another type of self‐report, one which has been adapted for research into the teaching and learning of communication skills of social work students specifically (e.g., Koprowska,  2010 ; Lefevre,  2010 ; Tompsett, Henderson, Gaskell Mew, et al.,  2017b ). They are inexpensive and easy to administer and analyse. However, the limitations of using self‐efficacy as an outcome measure are widely acknowledged (Drisko,  2014 ). Response‐shift bias is one limitation of self‐efficacy scales discussed in the literature, whereby some individuals may change their understanding of the concept being measured during the intervention. Such ‘contamination’ of self‐efficacy scores (Howard & Dailey,  1979 ) can mask the positive effects of the intervention. This may explain why no change was identified by Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ); however since a retrospective pre‐test was not issued to the students in their study, neither the presence nor impact of response‐shift bias can be established. Alternatively, the scales themselves may have contributed to the surprising results found by Rawlings ( 2008 ) and Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) since neither were properly validated. The subjectivity of self‐efficacy scales has been identified as another area of concern. Previous research has found that students’ self‐ratings do not necessarily correlate with those of field instructors/practice educators (Fortune et al.,  2005 ; Vitali,  2011 ), lecturers or service user‐actors (Koprowska,  2010 ). In this review, self‐efficacy scores and externally rated direct practice scores did not correlate in Rawlings ( 2008 ) study.

Self‐report instruments are still the most common way to measure empathy (Ilgunaite et al.,  2017 ; Segal et al.,  2017 ). However, the challenges associated with measuring perceived empathy through self‐reports (Lietz et al.,  2011 ; Robieux et al.,  2018 ) was clearly demonstrated in this review. Study authors anticipated that students’ perceived empathy levels would increase following training, but this expectation did not come to fruition in at least three studies, despite the study authors using different self‐report measures (including the IRI, QMEE and the TEQ), and even where other measures in the same studies did indicate skill gains. High and perhaps inflated ratings at pre‐test mask the improvements researchers anticipated. Greeno et al. ( 2017 ) acknowledged that training may impact more on behaviours and skills than self‐perception and identified that students’ TEQ scores were affected by high levels of perceived empathy at pre‐test. They suggested social desirability, meaning social work students want to be regarded as empathic, could compound this further, resulting in high rating scores at pre‐test. This ‘ceiling and testing effect’ (Greeno et al.,  2017 , p. 803) has been identified elsewhere (Gockel & Burton,  2014 ) and might result in a lack of significant changes in students’ level of reported empathy over time. Ilgunaite et al. ( 2017 , p. 14) also warn of social desirability, highlighting the controversy associated with asking people with poor empathic skills to self‐evaluate their own empathic abilities.

Concerns have been raised about what self‐reports actually measure, reflecting one type of conceptualisation at the expense of others. For example, the Toronto Empathy Questionnaire used in Greeno et al.'s ( 2017 ) study views empathy primarily as an emotional process but leaves the cognitive components of perspective taking and self/other awareness unaccounted for. This reflects wider concerns regarding the validity of self‐report questionnaires as an accurate measure of outcomes.

The finding that self‐report scores did not significantly correlate with other measures that were used alongside them lends support to the claim that empathic attitudes are not ‘a proxy for actions’ (Lietz et al.,  2011 , p. 104). It is possible that skills training has more impact on students’ behaviours than their attitudes, a point that was made by Barber ( 1988 ). Regardless of the varying explanations, self‐report measures of empathy tell us very little about empathic accuracy (Gerdes et al.,  2010 , p. 2334). The problems are not specific to the studies in this review or social work education in general. In an evaluation of empathy measurement tools used in nursing research, Yu and Kirk ( 2009 ) suggested that of the 12 measures they reviewed, none of them were ‘psychometrically and conceptually satisfactory’ (p. 1790).

Schinke et al.'s ( 1978 ) study bucked the trend, finding students’ positive attitudes towards their skills were almost three times higher among those who had received CST compared to those who did not. Interestingly, the self‐report instrument used in this study measured clearly specified counselling skills, and thus did not suffer from the conceptual confusion faced by those seeking to measure empathy.

Observer ratings

Observer ratings, conducted by independent raters, are often considered to be more valid and reliable measures of communication skills than the aforementioned subjective self‐report measures. Observation measures enable third party assessment of non‐verbal and verbal behaviours to be undertaken. As Keefe ( 1979 , p. 31) suggests, ‘accurate’ empathy when measured against a set of observer rating scales has been the basis for much valuable research and training in social work, particularly when combined with other variables. Observation measures were the primary instrument employed by the researchers of the included studies and produced the clearest demonstration of the effects of CST.

Studies using objective measures showed positive change, suggesting empathy training is effective. Studies using both self‐report and objective measures reported no significant changes in empathy using self‐report but found higher levels of behavioural empathy when using objective measures. The same pattern was identified in a review of empathy training by Teding van Berkhout and Malouff ( 2015 ). As Greeno et al. ( 2017 , p. 804) explain, perceived empathy is not correlated to actual empathic behaviours as scored by observers . Observation measures also posed some challenges for the studies included in this review, for example the repeated use of scales in training and assessment creates the problem of test‐retest artefacts (Nerdrum & Lundquist,  1995 ).

The Carkhuff ( 1969a ,  1969b ) scales have been frequently used in social work education (Hepworth et al.,  2010 ). The Carkhuff communication index is a written skills test measure used to assess the level of facilitative communication or core condition responses in relation to client statements of standardised vignettes. Carkhuff ( 1969a ) reported that there is a close relation between responses to the stimulus expressions in written form and verbal form and responses offered in an actual interview with a client. Thus, Carkhuff concludes that ‘both written and verbal responses to helpee stimulus expressions are valid indexes of assessments of the counselor in the actual helping role’ (Carkhuff,  1969a , p. 108). However, mastery of accurate discrimination has not been sufficient to guarantee congruent empathic responding within a given verbal interaction. Providing verbal empathic responses is arguably more challenging than producing written statements, hence in VanCleave's ( 2007 ) study, trained raters used the Carkhuff's Index for Communication to score the empathic responses of students to the Carkhuff stems, which were delivered by trained actors. Through comparing the findings produced by different methods of measurement, Collins ( 1984 ) found, ‘students were significantly better at writing minimally facilitative skill responses than demonstrating them orally as measured in a role‐play interview (p. 124). Noting ‘a lack of equivalence between written and oral modes of responding’, the validity of the Carkhuff stems is challenged by Collins’ study (Collins,  1984 , p. 148). Schinke et al. ( 1978 ) acknowledge similar concerns. Written skills test measures are not generalisable to, or indicative of, students’ behavioural responses in real life settings, threatening the ecological validity of such measures.

Vinton and Harrington ( 1994 ) also used the Carkhuff scale to measure expressed empathy and encountered measurement issues, which they suggest could have been caused by the validity of the measure, the additional statement they included in the questionnaire or other variables such as personality characteristics or background experiences.

The challenge of measuring empathy is apparent both within and across the included studies. Studies of empathy within social work have adopted a range of disparate methods to measure empathy depending on how it has been conceptualised (Lynch et al.,  2019 ; Pedersen,  2009 ), often focusing on one component of empathy at the expense of another. As Gerdes and Segal ( 2009 , p. 115) explain, ‘semantic fuzziness, conceptualizations and measurement techniques for empathy vary so much that it has been difficult to engage in meaningful comparisons or make significant conclusions about how we define empathy, measure it, and effectively cultivate it’.

6.3.3. Concerns about outcomes

The paucity of evidence‐supported outcome measures in social work education has been apparent for some time (Holden et al.,  2017 ), an issue we see reflected in this review.

Self‐efficacy

Self‐efficacy has been introduced as one means of assessing outcomes in social work education (Bell et al.,  2005 ; Holden et al.,  1997 ,  2002 ,  2005 ; Unrau & Grinnell,  2005 ). Self‐efficacy is deemed to be an important component of learning because ‘unless people believe they can produce desired effects by their actions, they have little incentive to act’ (Bandura,  1986 , p. 3). However, the use of self‐efficacy as an outcome measure in social work education is not without controversy, with some people recommending that ‘change in actual behaviours should be assessed where possible’ (Doyle et al.,  2011 , p. 105). Rawlings ( 2008 ) cautions against the use of self‐efficacy as a proxy measure for skill; ‘measures of social work self‐efficacy are limited to student beliefs or perception regarding skill and do not measure actual performance’ (pp. 7–8).

6.3.4. Concerns about research designs

The research designs used to investigate the effectiveness of interventions in social work education lack rigour, with few adhering to all the key features constituting a true experimental design. As Carpenter ( 2005 , p. 4) suggests, ‘the poor quality of research design of many studies, together with the limited information provided in the published accounts are major problems in establishing an evidence base for social work education’ (Carpenter,  2005 , p. 4). Identifying a dearth of writing which addressed the challenging issues of evaluating the learning and teaching of communication skills in social work education, Trevithick et al. ( 2004 , p. 28), in a UK‐based review, point out that ‘without robust evaluative strategies and studies the risks of fragmented and context restricted learning are heightened’. Similar issues arise in educational research more generally.

6.3.5. Concerns about researcher allegiance, positionality and confirmation bias

The study authors are predominantly social work academics conducting research within their own institutions. It is highly likely that they will have a vested interest in wanting the teaching of communication skills to be successful, particularly if they have been involved in the development of the intervention(s) under investigation. Researcher allegiance bias, and the challenges this presents are increasingly being recognised (Grundy et al.,  2020 ; Montgomery & Belle Weisman,  2021 ; Uttley & Montgomery,  2017 ; Yoder et al.,  2019 ). Whilst some risks of bias have been reduced within the included studies, they have not been eliminated. The relationships between students, academics and researchers, and the impact these dynamics may have on study findings, are largely under‐explored.

The studies included in this review are not large multi‐team trials, rather the study authors are working in small groups or alone, which hampers the resources available to them to mitigate bias in data collection and analysis procedures. Using an independent statistician to facilitate the blinding of outcome measures would have enabled study authors to overcome the inability to blind the participants or the experimenters.

Reviewers are no more immune from conflicts of interest or unconscious bias than the triallists and researchers of the included studies. Both reviewers are social work academics, and the first author (ERH) teaches communication skills to social work students, which is why it became the topic of her PhD. Whilst neither reviewer have a vested interest in any of the authors, institutions or interventions under investigation in the included studies, the first author acknowledges that she believes, or at least hopes, that students’ communication skills and their development of empathy, will be enhanced through taught interventions. ERH has had to be very mindful throughout the review of the potential for unconscious confirmation bias, and the need to remain as objective and impartial as possible. She also recognises that her own positionality, influenced by pedagogic experiences and social work values, have led her to believe in the importance of the educator's teaching style, the positive contribution of service user and carer involvement, and the added value of involving students in curriculum delivery and design, especially for developing social work skills (Reith‐Hall,  2020 ). These components were largely absent from the included studies, a source of frustration to the first author, who frequently has to remind herself that constructs of teaching and learning have changed considerably from when the majority of the included studies were undertaken, and that her views on such matters might be partly cultural and highly personal. Whilst unlikely to have affected the conduct or findings of the review itself, ERH recognises her beliefs have a bearing on the gaps identified in the research and potential policy and practice implications.

6.4. Potential biases in the review process

We performed a comprehensive search of a wide range of electronic databases and grey literature followed by the hand searching of key journals and reference searching of relevant studies. Both members of the review team screened all records and assessed all included studies against the inclusion criteria set out in the protocol, increasing consistency and rigour and minimising potential biases in the review process.

We sought to locate all publicly available studies on the effect of teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education during the review process, however it is difficult to establish if our endeavours were successful. It was a surprise to the first author that one of the included studies, which very clearly met the inclusion criteria, was obtained through reference searching, rather than through the electronic database search. As predicted by the second author, the age and style of the publication meant no key words were used, a search function upon which the electronic databases rely. Whilst this study came to light through reference searching, we cannot be entirely sure that other similar studies were surfaced in this way. Therefore publication bias cannot be entirely ruled out.

Our search was not limited to records written in English; indeed, one of the two unobtainable studies was written in Afrikaans, however, the rest of the studies were written in English. Rather than indicating a limitation of the way the review was conducted, it is likely that the location of the studies is responsible for the language bias—all of the included studies were conducted in English‐speaking countries, with the majority from the United States. Evidence‐based practice is well established in the United States, contributing to the use of study designs that increase the likelihood of them being included in systematic reviews.

Uncertainties and differences of opinion were resolved through contacting study authors for further information and through further reading and discussion, without recourse for a third‐party adjudicator. Both reviewers independently screened and assessed the studies. We are not aware of other potential biases or limitations inherent within the review process.

6.5. Agreements and disagreements with other studies or reviews

Findings from the included studies indicate that communication skills including empathy can be learned, and that the systematic training of student social workers produces improvements in their communication skills (Greeno et al.,  2017 ; Larsen & Hepworth,  1978 ; Laughlin,  1978 ; Pecukonis et al.,  2016 ; Schinke et al.,  1978 ; VanCleave,  2007 ), at least in the short term.

The findings of this systematic review broadly agree with the knowledge reviews about communication skills produced for the Social Care Institute of Excellence (Luckock et al.,  2006 ; Trevithick et al.,  2004 ). The knowledge reviews highlight that despite a lack of evidence, weak study designs, and a low level of rigour, study findings for the teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education are promising. Reviews of communication skills and empathy training in medical education (Aspegren,  1999 ; Batt‐Rawden et al.,  2013 ), where RCTs and validated outcome measures prevail, also suggest that CST leads to demonstrable improvements for students.

The findings from our review identified the same gaps as those found in the UK‐based social work knowledge and practice reviews for social work education, suggesting that little has changed. Trevithick et al. ( 2004 ) suggest that interventions are under‐theorised and the issue of whether students transfer their skills from the classroom to the workplace is unclear. Our findings concur with these observations. Diggins ( 2004 ) and Dinham ( 2006 ) identified the existence of far greater expertise and more examples of good practice than that reflected in the literature. Regrettably, our review suggests little has changed in almost 20 years.

7. AUTHORS’ CONCLUSIONS

7.1. implications for practice.

This review aimed to examine effects on a range of outcomes in social work education. With the exception of skill acquisition, there was insufficient evidence available to offer firm conclusions on other outcomes. It is unclear whether an issue with measurement or something to do with how students learn, or a combination of the two, is responsible for such uncertainty. Our understanding of how communication skills and empathy are learnt and taught remain limited, due to a lack of empirical research and comprehensive discussion. Discussing pedagogical explorations of empathy, Zaleski ( 2016 , p. 48) points out, ‘there lacks a sufficient exploration of specific teaching strategies’. Our review echoes and amplifies this view, within the context of social work education specifically. Disagreement remains within social work academia as to what empathy consists of. Segal et al. ( 2017 ) draw on cognitive neuroscience, and the role of mirror neurones, to underpin the teaching of empathy in social work education and practice. Eriksson and Englander ( 2017 , p. 607) take ‘a critical, phenomenological stance towards Gerdes and Segal's work’, exploring how empathy is conveyed in a context where practitioners are unlikely to be able to relate personally to the experiences of their client group. Given the continuing debate about the role of walking in someone else's shoes, it is hardly surprising that the studies in this review conceptualise and measure different aspects of empathy in a variety of ways producing incomplete and inconsistent results. Due to the clinical heterogeneity of populations and interventions, low methodological rigour and high risk of bias within the included studies, caution should be exercised when interpreting the findings for practice and policy.

Despite the limitations and variations in educational culture, the findings are still useful, and indicate that CST is likely to be beneficial. One important implication for practice appears to be that the teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education should provide opportunities for students to practice skills in a simulated (or real) environment. Toseland and Spielberg ( 1982 ) suggest that skills diminish gradually if not reinforced. They suggest that students should be exposed to the effective application of interpersonal helping skills in several different courses and be encouraged to practice these skills in a variety of case situations role‐played in classroom and laboratory settings, as well as in field settings. Larsen and Hepworth ( 1978 ) and Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) also suggest that CST must be better integrated with practice settings, where students can demonstrate communicative and interviewing abilities with actual clients in real‐world practice settings, ‘the ultimate test of any social work practice skill’ (Schinke et al.,  1978 , p. 400).

Technology is widely used in the teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education, and whilst technological advances have been considerable in recent years, current practice is not captured in the studies featuring in this review. The further sharing of good practice between students and educators continues to be necessary. The Australian Association of Social Workers identifies that face‐to‐face teaching remains the standard approach for teaching professional practice skills, whilst acknowledging that online technologies and blended learning are also encouraged (Australian Association of Social Workers,  2020 ). Barriers preventing the further uptake of technology throughout social work education have been identified. In a review of the literature into key issues with web‐based learning in human services, Moore ( 2005 ) discovered that some social work educators believe traditional instruction to be superior to web‐based instruction, especially for courses focused on micro practice and clinical skills. Similar findings have been reproduced more recently, especially for practice‐oriented competencies (Levin et al.,  2018 ). Despite such reservations, reviews into technology‐based pedagogical methods in social work education have indicated that students’ competencies were largely equitable between online and face‐to‐face modalities (Afrouz & Crisp,  2021 ; Wretman & Macy,  2016 ). The extent to which this applies to outcomes of communication skills and empathy remains unknown. In this review the studies that compared face‐to‐face interventions with online interventions did not reach a consensus, since Ouellette et al. ( 2006 ) found there was no difference in outcomes between online and face‐to‐face teaching, whilst Greeno et al. ( 2017 ) and Pecukonis et al. ( 2016 ) found the outcomes of students who received live supervision were greater than those who engaged in self‐directed study online. However, we do not know whether student outcomes were affected by the presence or absence of an educator. Differences might not be attributable to the interventions themselves, for as Levin et al. ( 2018 , p. 777) remark, ‘the role of an instructor in online learning cannot be underestimated’.

Certainly, the proliferation of online social work courses is evident across Australia (Australian Association of Social Workers,  2018 ) and the USA (Council on Social Work Education,  2020 ). The global coronavirus/Covid‐19 pandemic has led to exponential growth of online teaching and learning in social work education, hence ‘we can be nearly certain that the ‘new normal’ will include the use of information technology’ (Tedam & Tedam,  2020 , p. 3). Therefore, it is imperative that we investigate the impact of online learning and web‐based instruction and the role of the educator in different contexts on the development of social work students’ communicative and empathic abilities.

7.2. Implications for research

There is much to be done to improve the outcome studies in social work education generally and for the teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education specifically. Robust study designs that support causal inferences through the random allocation to intervention and control groups is a necessity. Steps to reduce threats to the internal validity of case‐controlled studies should also be exercised to reduce the impact of test–retest artefacts identified by Nerdrum and Lundquist ( 1995 ) in some of the other studies. More work is needed on defining and measuring outcomes (Diggins,  2004 ). Validated measures which can be used consistently across future studies would make comparisons easier and enable future synthesis to be more meaningful.

The review found that relying solely on self‐report measures was problematic, particularly given that the findings from these did not correlate with the findings produced from other measures. Vinton and Harrington ( 1994 ) found there was no statistically significant correlation between students’ perceptions of their learning experience and self‐assessment of their skill acquisition with the independent evaluator's rating of the students’ acquisition of interviewing skills. Methodological triangulation should be considered in future studies.

Other study authors advise researchers to use objective measures of communication skills including behavioural measures of empathy (Greeno et al.,  2017 ; Pecukonis et al.,  2016 ), a recommendation also made by Teding van Berkhout and Malouff ( 2015 ) in a review of empathy training. Collins ( 1984 ) recommended that more research is required on the equivalency of measures, given the different results the measures in his study produced. Carpenter ( 2005 ,  2011 ) provides guidance on how research designs and outcome measures can be further developed in social work education. This review highlights the need for research that utilises follow‐up studies, which would help determine the extent to which training benefits endure after the end of training (Schinke et al.,  1978 ; VanCleave,  2007 ). Rawlings ( 2008 ) advises that a longitudinal design, testing the same students over time, is required. The need to investigate whether or not students were able to transfer their skills into practice has also been firmly stated (Carpenter,  2005 ).

In addition to outcome studies, VanCleave ( 2007 ) recommends the inclusion of qualitative data in researching the teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education. Building a qualitative strand into the research design would facilitate exploration and explanation of the quantitative outcomes. It would also enable the voices of the intended beneficiaries of the interventions under investigation to be heard and acted upon. As a values‐based profession, a focus on stakeholder participation and contribution should be at the forefront of research in social work education. The benefits of involving service users and carers in social work education are well rehearsed, and examples of their input in the teaching and learning of communication skills are plentiful within the wider literature. However, the value of service users and carers is not evident within the included studies, thus gap‐mending strategies need to be established across the realms of social work education, practice and research, to prevent certain types of social work knowledge receiving more preferential status than others. As Carpenter ( 2005 , p. 7) points out, since the purpose of the whole exercise is to benefit service users and/or carers, a comprehensive evaluation should ask whether training has made any difference to their lives’.

Finally, the theory of change appears to be assumed rather than clearly defined. Research that identifies the relevant substantive theories on which the teaching and learning of communication skills is based would provide a good starting point. Moreover, whilst the studies in the review indicated that CST encourages some improvement, particularly in terms of the skills outcomes measured, clarity on the mechanisms involved in positive effects requires additional research. The role of reflection, whilst briefly mentioned in some of the included studies, has been largely overlooked. The role of context is almost completely absent in the existing body of literature. Zaleski ( 2016 ) suggest the teaching style of the educator can influence students’ ability to learn empathy, yet they acknowledge that literature into the educational environment is lacking. A realist synthesis would support the theoretical development of the teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education. Realist synthesis is an interpretive theory‐driven methodological approach to reviewing quantitative, qualitative and mixed methods research evidence about complex social interventions to provide an explanatory analysis of how and why they work in particular contexts or settings. This research approach would support the theoretical development of the teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education, complementing that of this systematic review (Reith‐Hall,  2022 ).

CONTRIBUTIONS OF AUTHORS

  • Content: Emma Reith‐Hall
  • Systematic review methods: Emma Reith‐Hall and Paul Montgomery
  • Statistical analysis: Paul Montgomery
  • Information retrieval: Emma Reith‐Hall
  • Write up: Emma Reith‐Hall and Paul Montgomery

DECLARATIONS OF INTEREST

Emma Reith‐Hall is a social work academic who has been involved in the teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education in a number of higher education institutions. The author acknowledges she holds a position whereby she believes that communication skills can, and should, be taught, learnt, and refined. Paul Montgomery is primarily a methodologist and systematic reviewer who considers his position on the issue of communication skills to be equivocal. Neither author has a financial conflict of interest.

DIFFERENCES BETWEEN PROTOCOL AND REVIEW

Sources of support.

Internal sources

No internal sources of support.

External sources

ERH is undertaking the systematic review as part of her PhD research, for which she receives ESRC DTP funding (Grant number: ES/P000711/1).

  • Paul Montgomery, UK

No sources of support

Supporting information

Supporting information.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We are particularly grateful to our stakeholders—the students, practitioners, people with lived experience, social work academics and social work organisations who gave their input into the development of this systematic review. The contribution of two research‐minded social work students—Ryan Barber and Fee Steane is particularly appreciated.

Thank you to the editorial team at Campbell.

Emma Reith‐Hall is in receipt of an ESRC studentship, for which she receives ESRC funding.

Reith‐Hall, E. , & Montgomery, P. (2023). Communication skills training for improving the communicative abilities of student social workers . Campbell Systematic Reviews , 19 , e1309. 10.1002/cl2.1309 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

INCLUDED STUDIES

  • *Barber, J. (1988). Are microskills worth teaching? Journal of social work education , 24 ( 1 ), 3–12. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Collins, D. (1984). A study of the transfer of interviewing skills from the classroom to the field [Doctoral dissertation, University of Toronto].
  • Greeno, E. J. , Ting, L. , Pecukonis, E. , Hodorowicz, M. , & Wade, K. (2017). The role of empathy in training social work students in motivational interviewing . Social Work Education , 36 ( 7 ), 794–808. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hettinga, P. (1978). The impact of videotaped interview playback with instructional feedback on social work student self‐perceived interviewing competence and self‐esteem [Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota].
  • Keefe, T. (1979). The development of empathic skill: A study . Journal of Education for Social Work , 15 ( 2 ), 30–37. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Larsen, J. (1975). A comparative study of traditional and competency‐based methods of teaching interpersonal skills in social work education [Doctoral Dissertation, The University of Utah].
  • *Larsen, J. , & Hepworth, D. H. (1978). Skill development through competency‐based education . Journal of Education for Social Work , 14 ( 1 ), 73–81. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Laughlin, S. G. (1978). Use of self‐instruction in teaching empathic responding to social work students [Doctoral Dissertation, University of California].
  • *Ouellette, P. M. , Westhuis, D. , Marshall, E. , & Chang, V. (2006). The acquisition of social work interviewing skills in a web‐based and classroom instructional environment: results of a study . Journal of Technology in Human Services , 24 ( 4 ), 53–75. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pecukonis, E. , Greeno, E. , Hodorowicz, M. , Park, H. , Ting, L. , Moyers, T. , Burry, C. , Linsenmeyer, D. , Strieder, F. , Wade, K. , & Wirt, C. (2016). Teaching motivational interviewing to child welfare social work students using live supervision and standardized clients: A randomized controlled trial . Journal of the Society for Social Work and Research , 7 ( 3 ), 479–505. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Rawlings, M. A. (2008). Assessing direct practice skill performance in undergraduate social work education using standardized clients and self‐reported self‐efficacy [Doctoral dissertation, Case Western Reserve University].
  • *Schinke, S. P. , Smith, T. E. , Gilchrist, L. D. , & Wong, S. E. (1978). Interviewing‐skills training: An empirical evaluation . Journal of Social Service Research , 1 ( 4 ), 391–401. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Toseland, R. , & Spielberg, G. (1982). The development of helping skills in undergraduate social work education: Model and evaluation . Journal of Education for Social Work , 18 ( 1 ), 66–73. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *VanCleave, D. (2007). Empathy training for master's level social work students facilitating advanced empathy responding [Doctoral dissertation: Capella University].
  • *Vinton, L. , & Harrington, P. (1994). An evaluation of the use of videotape in teaching empathy . Journal of Teaching in Social Work , 9 ( 1–2 ), 71–84. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Wells, R. A. (1976). A comparison of role‐play and “own‐problem” procedures in systematic facilitative training . Psychotherapy: Theory, Research & Practice , 13 ( 3 ), 280–281. [ Google Scholar ]

EXCLUDED STUDIES

  • *Andrews, P. , & Harris, S. (2017). Using live supervision to teach counselling skills to social work students . Social Work Education , 36 ( 3 ), 299–311. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Bakx, A. W. E. A. , Van Der Sanden, J. M. M. , Sijtsma, K. , Croon, M. A. , & Vermetten, Y. J. M. (2006). The role of students’ personality characteristics, self‐perceived competence and learning conceptions in the acquisition and development of social communicative competence: A longitudinal study . Higher Education , 51 ( 1 ), 71–104. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Barclay, B. (2012). Undergraduate social work students: Learning interviewing skills in a hybrid practice class [Doctoral dissertation, Colorado State University].
  • *Bogo, M. , Regehr, C. , Baird, S. , Paterson, J. , & LeBlanc, V. R. (2017). Cognitive and affective elements of practice confidence in social work students and practitioners . British Journal of Social Work , 47 ( 3 ), 701–718. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Bolger, J. (2014). Video self‐modelling and its impact on the development of communication skills within social work education . Journal of Social Work , 14 ( 2 ), 196–212. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bolger, J. (2014). Video self‐modelling and its impact on the development of communication skills within social work education . Journal of Social Work , 14 ( 2 ), 196–212. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Carrillo, D. F. , & Thyer, B. A. (1994). Advanced standing and two‐year program MSW students: An empirical investigation of foundation interviewing skills . Journal of Social Work Education , 30 ( 3 ), 377–387. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Carillo, D. , Gallart, J. , & Thyer, B. (1993). Training MSW students in interviewing skills: An empirical assessment . Arete , 18 ( 1 ), 12–19. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Carter, K. , Swanke, J. , Stonich, J. , Taylor, S. , Witzke, M. , & Binetsch, M. (2018). Student assessment of self‐efficacy and practice readiness following simulated instruction in an undergraduate social work program . Journal of Teaching in Social Work , 38 ( 1 ), 28–42. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Cartney, P. (2006). Using video interviewing in the assessment of social work communication skills . British Journal of Social Work , 36 ( 5 ), 827–844. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Cetingok, M. (1988). Simulation group exercises and development of interpersonal skills: Social work administration students’ assessment in a simple time‐series design framework . Small Group Behavior , 19 ( 3 ), 395–404. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Collins, D. , Gabor, P. , & Ing, C. (1987). Communication skill training in child‐care: The effects of preservice and inservice training . Child & Youth Care Quarterly , 16 ( 2 ), 106–115. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Corcoran, J. , Stuart, S. , & Schultz, J. (2019). Teaching interpersonal psychotherapy (IPT) in an MSW clinical course . Journal of Teaching in Social Work , 39 ( 3 ), 226–236. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Domakin, A. (2013). Can online discussions help student social workers learn when studying communication? Social Work Education , 32 ( 1 ), 81–99. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Gockel, A. , & Burton, D. L. (2014). An evaluation of prepracticum helping skills training for graduate social work students . Journal of social work education , 50 ( 1 ), 101–119. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Hansen, F. C. B. , Resnick, H. , & Galea, J. (2002). Better listening: paraphrasing and perception checking—A study of the effectiveness of a multimedia skills training program . Journal of Technology in Human Services , 20 ( 3–4 ), 317–331. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Hodorowicz, M. (2018). Teaching and learning motivational interviewing: Examining the efficacy of two training methods for social work students [Doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland, Baltimore].
  • *Hodorowicz, M. T. , Barth, R. , Moyers, T. , & Strieder, F. (2020). A randomized controlled trial of two methods to improve motivational interviewing training . Research on Social Work Practice , 30 ( 4 ), 382–391. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Hohman, M. , Pierce, P. , & Barnett, E. (2015). Motivational interviewing: An evidence‐based practice for improving student practice skills . Journal of social work education , 51 ( 2 ), 287–297. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Kopp, J. (1982). Changes in graduate social work students’ use of interviewing skills from training to practicum [Doctoral Dissertation, Washington University in St. Louis].
  • *Kopp, J. , & Butterfield, W. (1985). Changes in graduate students’ use of interviewing skills from the classroom to the field . Journal of Social Service Research , 9 ( 1 ), 65–88. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Kopp, J. (1990). The transfer of interviewing skills to practicum by students with high and low pre‐training skill levels . Journal of Teaching in Social Work , 4 ( 1 ), 31–52. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Koprowska, J. (2010). Are student social workers’ communication skills improved by university‐based learning. In Burgess H., & Carpenter J. (Eds.), The outcomes of social work education: Developing evaluation methods (pp. 73–97). The Higher Education Academy; Social Policy and Social Work. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Lefevre, M. (2010). Evaluating the teaching and learning of communication skills for use with children and young people. In Burgess H., & Carpenter J. (Eds.), The outcomes of social work education: Developing evaluation methods (pp. 96–110). The Higher Education Academy; Social Policy and Social Work. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Magill, J. , & Werk, A. (1985). Classroom training as preparation for the social work practicum: An evaluation of a skills laboratory training program . The Clinical Supervisor , 3 ( 3 ), 69–76. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Mishna, F. , Tufford, L. , Cook, C. , & Bogo, M. (2013). Research note—A pilot cyber counseling course in a graduate social work program . Journal of Social Work Education , 49 ( 3 ), 515–524. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Nerdrum, P. (1997). Maintenance of the effect of training in communication skills: A controlled follow‐up study of level of communicated empathy . British Journal of Social Work , 27 ( 5 ), 705–722. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Nerdrum, P. , & Høglend, P. (2003). Short and long‐term effects of training in empathic communication: Trainee personality makes a difference . The Clinical Supervisor , 21 ( 2 ), 1–19. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Nerdrum, P. , & Lundquist, K. (1995). Does participation in communication skills training increase student levels of communicated empathy? A controlled outcome study . Journal of Teaching in Social Work , 11 ( 1–2 ), 139–157. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Patton, T. (2019). Engaging methods to teach empathy: A successful journey to transformation [Doctoral dissertation, Union University].
  • *Rogers, A. , & Welch, B. (2009). Using standardized clients in the classroom: An evaluation of a training module to teach active listening skills to social work students . Journal of Teaching in Social Work , 29 ( 2 ), 153–168. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Scannapieco, M. , Bolen, R. M. , & Connell, K. K. (2000). Professional social work education in child welfare: Assessing practice knowledge and skills . The International Journal of Continuing Social Work Education , 3 ( 1 ), 44–56. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Tompsett, H. , Henderson, K. , Mathew Byrne, J. , Gaskell Mew, E. , & Tompsett, C. (2017). Self‐efficacy and outcomes: Validating a measure comparing social work students’ perceived and assessed ability in core pre‐placement skills . The British Journal of Social Work , 47 ( 8 ), 2384–2405. [ Google Scholar ]
  • *Wodarski, J. S. , Pippin, J. A. , & Daniels, M. (1988). The effects of graduate social work education on personality, values and interpersonal skills . Journal of social work education , 24 ( 3 ), 266–277. [ Google Scholar ]
  • ADDITIONAL REFERENCES
  • Afrouz, R. , & Crisp, B. R. (2021). Online education in social work, effectiveness, benefits, and challenges: A scoping review . Australian Social Work , 74 ( 1 ), 55–67. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Askheim, O. P. , Beresford, P. , & Heule, C. (2017). Mend the gap—Strategies for user involvement in social work education . Social Work Education , 36 ( 2 ), 128–140. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Aspegren, K. (1999). BEME Guide No. 2: Teaching and learning communication skills in medicine—A review with quality grading of articles . Medical Teacher , 21 ( 6 ), 563–570. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Australian Association of Social Workers . (2018). AASW accredited courses . http://www.aasw.asn.au/careers-study/accredited-courses
  • Australian Association of Social Workers . (2020). Australian social work education and accreditation standards . https://www.aasw.asn.au/document/item/12845
  • Ayling, P. (2012). Learning through playing in higher education: promoting play as a skill for social work students . Social Work Education , 31 ( 6 ), 764–777. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Banach, M. , Rataj, A. , Ralph, M. , & Allosso, L. (2020). Learning social work through role play: Developing more confident and capable social workers . The Journal of Practice Teaching and Learning , 17 ( 1 ), 42–60. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bandura, A. (1971). Social learning theory . General Learning Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bandura, A. (1976). Self‐reinforcement: theoretical and methodological considerations . Behaviorism , 4 , 135–155. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bandura, A. (1982). Self‐efficacy mechanism in human agency . American Psychologist , 37 , 122–147. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory . Prentice‐Hall. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bandura, A. (1997). Self‐efficacy: Thought control of action . W.H. Freeman and Company. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective . Annual Review of Psychology , 52 , 1–26. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barak, A. , & LaCrosse, M. B. (1975). Multidimensional perception of counselor behavior . Journal of Counseling Psychology , 22 ( 6 ), 471–476. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barr, H. , Freeth, D. , Hammick, M. , Koppel, I. , & Reeves, S. (2000). Evaluating interprofessional education: A United Kingdom review for health and social care . Centre for the Advancement of Interprofessional Education. https://www.caipe.org
  • Batt‐Rawden, S. A. , Chisolm, M. S. , Anton, B. , & Flickinger, T. E. (2013). Teaching empathy to medical students: An updated, systematic review . Academic Medicine , 88 ( 8 ), 1171–1177. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Beesley, P. , Watts, M. , & Harrison, M. (2018). Developing your communication skills in social work . Sage. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bell, S. A. , Rawlings, M. , & Johnson, B. (2005). Assessing skills, attitudes, and knowledge in gerontology: The results of an infused curriculum project . Journal of Baccalaureate Social Work , 11 ( sp1 ), 26–37. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Beresford, P. , Croft, S. , & Adshead, L. (2008). ‘We don't see her as a social worker’: A service user case study of the importance of the social worker's relationship and humanity . British Journal of Social Work , 38 ( 7 ), 1388–1407. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Boutron, I. , Page, M. J. , Higgins, J. P. T. , Altman, D. G. , Lundh, A. , & Hróbjartsson, A. (2021). Chapter 7: Considering bias and conflicts of interest among the included studies. In Higgins J. P. T., Thomas J., Chandler J., Cumpston M., Li T., Page M. J., & Welch V. A. (Eds.), Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 6.2 (updated February 2021) . Cochrane. [ Google Scholar ]
  • British Association of Social Workers . (2018). Professional capabilities framework for social work in England: The 2018 refreshed PCF . https://www.basw.co.uk/system/files/resources/BASW%20PCF.%20Detailed%20level%20descriptors%20for%20all%20domains.25.6.18%20final.pdf
  • Brunero, S. , Lamont, S. , & Coates, M. (2010). A review of empathy education in nursing . Nursing Inquiry , 17 ( 1 ), 65–74. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Campbell, D. T. , & Stanley, J. (1963). Experimental and quasi‐experimental designs for research on teaching. In Gage N. L. (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching . (Vol. 5 , pp. 171–246). Rand McNally. [ Google Scholar ]
  •  Campbell, R. J. , Kagan, N. , & Krathwohl, D. R. (1971). The development and validation of a scale to measure affective sensitivity (empathy) . Journal of Counseling Psychology , 18 ( 5 ), 407–412. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carkhuff, R. R. , & Truax, C. B. (1965). Training in counseling and psychotherapy: An evaluation of an integrated didactic and experiential approach . Journal of Consulting Psychology , 29 ( 4 ), 333–336. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carkhuff, R. R. (1969a). Helping and human relations. Vol. I: Selection and training . Holt, Rinehart and Winston. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carkhuff, R. R. (1969b). Helping and human relations. Vol. II: Practice and research . Holt, Rinehart and Winston. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carkhuff, R. R. (1969c). Helping and human relations: A primer for lay and professional helpers . Holt, Rhinehart & Winston. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carkhuff, R. R. , & Berenson, B. G. (1976). Teaching as treatment: An introduction to counseling & psychotherapy . Human Resource Development Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carpenter, J. (2005). Evaluating outcomes in social work education: Evaluation and evidence (Discussion Paper 1). SCIE. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carpenter, J. (2011). Evaluating social work education: A review of outcomes, measures, research designs and practicalities . Social Work Education , 30 ( 2 ), 122–140. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carpenter, J. (2016). Evaluating the outcomes of social work education. In Taylor I., Bogo M., Lefevre M., & Teater B. (Eds.), Routledge international handbook of social work education . Routledge. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cartney, P. (2006). Using video interviewing in the assessment of social work communication skills . British Journal of Social Work , 36 , 827–844. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chang, V. , & Scott, S. T. (1999). Basic interviewing skills: A workbook for practitioners . Nelson‐Hall Publishers. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cochrane Effective Practice and Organisation of Care . (2017). What study designs can be considered for inclusion in an EPOC review and what should they be called? EPOC Resources for review authors . http://epoc.cochrane.org/resources/epoc-resources-review-authors
  • Council on Social Work Education . (2015). Education policy and accreditation standards . https://www.cswe.org/getattachment/Accreditation/Accreditation-Process/2015-EPAS/2015EPAS_Web_FINAL.pdf.aspx
  • Council on Social Work Education . (2020). Statistics on social work education in the United States: Summary of the CSWE annual survey of social work programs . https://www.cswe.org/getattachment/Research-Statistics/2019-Annual-Statisticson-Social-Work-Education-in-the-United-States-Final-(1).pdf.aspx
  • Cournoyer, B. (2016). The social work skills workbook (8th ed.). Cengage. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy . JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents of Psychology , 10 , 85. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Department of Health . (2002). Focus on the future: Key messages from focus groups about the future of social work education . Department of Health. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Diggins, M. (2004). Teaching and learning communication skills in social work education . SCIE Guide , 5 , 1–77. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dinham, A. (2006). A review of practice of teaching and learning of communication skills in social work education in England . Social Work Education , 25 ( 8 ), 838–850. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Doyle, D. , Copeland, H. L. , Bush, D. , Stein, L. , & Thompson, S. (2011). A course for nurses to handle difficult communication situations. A randomized controlled trial of impact on self‐efficacy and performance . Patient Education and Counseling , 82 ( 1 ), 100–109. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Drisko, J. W. (2014). Competencies and their assessment . Journal of social work education , 50 , 414–426. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dupper, D. (2017). Strengthening empathy training programs for undergraduate social work students . Journal of Baccalaureate Social Work , 22 ( 1 ), 31–41. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Edwards, J. B. , & Richards, A. (2002). Relational teaching: A view of relational teaching in social work education . Journal of Teaching in Social Work , 22 , 33–48. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eisner, M. (2009). No effects in independent prevention trials: Can we reject the cynical view? Journal of Experimental Criminology , 5 ( 2 ), 163–183. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Elliott, R. , Bohart, A. C. , Watson, J. C. , & Murphy, D. (2018). Therapist empathy and client outcome: An updated meta‐analysis . Psychotherapy , 55 ( 4 ), 399–410. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eraut, M. (1994). Developing professional knowledge and competence . Falmer. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eriksson, K. , & Englander, M. (2017). Empathy in social work . Journal of social work education , 53 ( 4 ), 607–621. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ferguson, H. (2016). What social workers do in performing child protection work: evidence from research into face‐to‐face practice . Child & Family Social Work , 21 ( 3 ), 283–294. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Forrester, D. , Kershaw, S. , Moss, H. , & Hughes, L. (2008). Communication skills in child protection: how do social workers talk to parents? Child and Family Social Work , 38 , 1302–1319. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fortune, A. E. , Lee, M. , & Cavazos, A. (2005). Achievement motivation and outcome in social work field education . Journal of Social Work Education , 41 ( 1 ), 115–129. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gagnier, J. J. , Morgenstern, H. , Altman, D. G. , Berlin, J. , Chang, S. , McCulloch, P. , Sun, X. , & Moher, D. (2013). Consensus‐based recommendations for investigating clinical heterogeneity in systematic reviews . BMC Medical Research Methodology , 13 ( 1 ), 106. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gair, S. (2011). Creating spaces for critical reflection in social work education: Learning from a classroom‐based empathy project . Reflective Practice , 12 ( 6 ), 791–802. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gerdes, K. E. , & Segal, E. A. (2009). A social work model of empathy . Advances in Social Work , 10 ( 2 ), 114–127. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gerdes, K. E. , & Segal, E. (2011). Importance of empathy for social work practice: integrating new science . Social Work , 56 ( 2 ), 141–148. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gerdes, K. E. , Segal, E. A. , & Lietz, C. A. (2010). Conceptualising and measuring empathy . British Journal of Social Work , 40 ( 7 ), 2326–2343. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grant, S. , Mayo‐Wilson, E. , Montgomery, P. , Macdonald, G. , Michie, S. , Hopewell, S. , & Moher, D. (2018). CONSORT‐SPI 2018 explanation and elaboration: Guidance for reporting social and psychological intervention trials . Trials , 19 ( 1 ), 406. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grundy, Q. , Mayes, C. , Holloway, K. , Mazzarello, S. , Thombs, B. D. , & Bero, L. (2020). Conflict of interest as ethical shorthand: Understanding the range and nature of “non‐financial conflict of interest” in biomedicine . Journal of Clinical Epidemiology , 120 , 1–7. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Handley, G. , & Doyle, C. (2014). Ascertaining the wishes and feelings of young children: social workers' perspectives on skills and training: Ascertaining children's views . Child & Family Social Work , 19 ( 4 ), 443–454. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hargie, O. (2006). The handbook of communication skills . Routledge. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hargie, O. (2017). Skilled interpersonal communication: Research, theory and practice (6th ed.). Routledge. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Harms, L. (2015). Working with people: Communication skills for reflective practice (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Healy, K. (2018). The skilled communicator in social work: The art and science of communication in practice . Palgrave. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hemmerdinger, J. M. , Stoddart, S. D. , & Lilford, R. J. (2007). A systematic review of tests of empathy in medicine . BMC Medical Education , 7 ( 1 ), 24. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hepworth, D. H. , Rooney, R. H. , Rooney, G. D. , Strom‐Gottfried, K. , & Larsen, J. (2010). Direct social work practice: Theory and skills (8th ed.). Brooks/Cole. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Higgins, J. P. T. , Savović, J. , Page, M. J. , & Sterne, J. A. C. (2019). The Revised Cochrane risk‐of‐bias tool for randomized trials (RoB 2) . https://drive.google.com/open?id=19R9savfPdCHC8XLz2iiMvL_71lPJERWK
  • Higgins, J. P. T. , Li, T. , & Deeks, J. J. (Eds.). (2022). Chapter 6: Choosing effect measures and computing estimates of effect. In Higgins, J. P. T. , Thomas, J. , Chandler, J. , Cumpston, M. , Li, T. , Page, M. J. & Welch, V. A. (Eds.), Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of Interventions version 6.3 . Cochrane. Retrieved June 16, 2022, from www.training.cochrane.org/handbook
  • Higgins, J. P. T. , Thomas, J. , Chandler, J. , Cumpston, M. , Li, T. , Page, M. J. , & Welch, V. A. (2021). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 6.2 . www.training.cochrane.org/handbook [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ]
  • Holden, G. , Cuzzi, L. , Spitzer, W. , Rutter, S. , Chernack, P. , & Rosenberg, G. (1997). The hospital social work self‐efficacy scale: A partial replication and extension . Health & Social Work , 22 ( 4 ), 256–263. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Holden, G. , Meenaghan, T. , Anastas, J. , & Metrey, G. (2002). Outcomes of social work education: The case for social work self‐efficacy . Journal of Social Work Education , 38 ( 1 ), 115–133. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Holden, G. , Anastas, J. , & Meenaghan, T. (2005). Research notes:EPAS objectives and foundation practice self‐efficacy: A replication . Journal of Social Work Education , 41 ( 3 ), 559–570. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Holden, G. , Barker, K. , Kuppens, S. , & Rosenberg, G. (2017). Self‐efficacy regarding social work competencies . Research on Social Work Practice , 27 ( 5 ), 594–606. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Howard, G. S. , & Dailey, P. R. (1979). Response‐shift bias: A source of contamination of self‐report measures . Journal of Applied Psychology , 64 ( 2 ), 144–150. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Huerta‐Wong, J. E. , & Schoech, R. (2010). Experiential learning and learning environments: The case of active listening skills . Journal of Social Work Education , 46 ( 1 ), 85–101. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ilgunaite, G. , Giromini, L. , & Di Girolamo, M. (2017). Measuring empathy: A literature review of available tools . BPA—Applied Psychology Bulletin , 65 , 280. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ingram, R. (2013). Locating emotional intelligence at the heart of social work practice . British Journal of Social Work , 43 ( 5 ), 987–1004. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ivey, A. E. , & Authier, J. (1971). Microcounseling: Innovation in interviewing training . Charles C. Thomas. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ivey, A. E. , Normington, C. J. , Miller, C. D. , Morrill, W. H. , & Haase, R. F. (1968). Microcounseling and attending behavior: An approach to prepracticum counselor training . Journal of Counseling Psychology , 15 , 1–12. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kadushin, A. , & Kadushin, G. (2013). The social work interview (5th ed.). Columbia University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kam, P. K. (2020). ‘Social work is not just a job’: The qualities of social workers from the perspective of service users . Journal of Social Work , 20 ( 6 ), 775–796. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kirkpatrick, D. L. (1967). Evaluation of training. In Craig R. L., & Bittel L. R. (Eds.), Training and development handbook (pp. 87–112). McGraw‐Hill. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Knowles, M. S. (1972). Innovations in teaching styles and approaches based upon adult learning . Journal of Education for Social Work , 8 , 32–39. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Knowles, M. (1998). The adult learner . Gulf Publishing Company. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kolb, D. (1984). Experiential learning: Experience as the source of learning and development . Prentice‐Hall. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koprowska, J. (2003). The right kind of telling? Locating the teaching of interviewing skills within a systems framework . British Journal of Social Work , 33 , 291–308. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koprowska, J. (2010). The outcomes of social work education: Developing evaluation methods . Higher Education Academy, SWAP. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koprowska, J. (2020). Communication and interpersonal skills in social work (5th ed.). Learning Matters: Sage. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kraiger, K. , Ford, J. K. , & Salas, E. (1993). Application of cognitive, skill‐based, and affective theories of learning outcomes to new methods of training evaluation . Journal of Applied Psychology , 78 ( 2 ), 311–328. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kruger, J. , & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self‐assessments . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 77 ( 6 ), 1121–1134. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kugley, S. , Wade, A. , Thomas, J. , Mahood, Q. , Jørgensen, A. M. K. , Hammerstrøm, K. , & Sathe, N. (2017). Searching for studies: A guide to information retrieval for Campbell . Campbell Systematic Reviews , 13 ( 1 ), 1–73. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lam, T. C. M. , Kolomitro, K. , & Alamparambil, F. C. (2011). Empathy training: Methods, evaluation practices, and validity . Journal of Multidisciplinary Evaluation , 7 ( 16 ), 162–200. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Laming, H. (2003). The Victoria Climbie Inquiry: Report of an inquiry by Lord Laming . https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/the-victoria-climbie-inquiry-report-of-an-inquiry-by-lord-laming
  • Laming, H. (2009). The protection of children in England: A progress report . https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/the-protection-of-children-in-england-a-progress-report
  • Larson, L. M. , & Daniels, J. A. (1998). Review of the counseling self‐efficacy literature . The Counseling Psychologist , 26 ( 2 ), 179–218. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lefevre, M. , Tanner, K. , & Luckock, B. (2008). Developing social work students’ communication skills with children and young people: A model for the qualifying level curriculum . Child & Family Social Work , 13 , 166–176. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lefevre, M. (2010). The outcomes of social work education: Developing evaluation methods . Higher Education Academy, SWAP. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Levin, S. , Fulginiti, A. , & Moore, B. (2018). The perceived effectiveness of online social work education: Insights from a national survey of social work educators . Social Work Education , 37 ( 6 ), 775–789. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lietz, C. A. , Gerdes, K. E. , Sun, F. , Geiger, J. M. , Wagaman, M. A. and Segal, E. A. (2011). The Empathy Assessment Index (EAI): A confirmatory factor analysis of a multidimensional model of empathy . Journal of the Society for Social Work and Research , 2 ( 2 ), 1–202. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lishman, J. (2009). Communication in social work (2nd ed.). Palgrave Macmillan. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Luckock, B. , Lefevre, M. , Orr, D. , Jones, M. , Marchant, R. , & Tanner, K. (2006). Teaching, learning and assessing communication skills with children and young people in social work education . Knowledge Review , 1–202. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lynch, A. , Newlands, F. , & Forrester, D. (2019). What does empathy sound like in social work communication? A mixed‐methods study of empathy in child protection social work practice . Child & Family Social Work , 24 , 139–147. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maynard, B. R. , Solis, M. R. , Miller, V. L. , & Brendel, K. E. (2017). Mindfulness‐based interventions for improving cognition, academic achievement, behavior, and socioemotional functioning of primary and secondary school students . Campbell Systematic Reviews , 13 ( 1 ), 1–144. 10.4073/2017.5 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mehrabian, A. , & Epstein, N. (1972). A measure of emotional empathy . Journal of Personality , 40 , 523–543. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Montgomery, P. , & Belle Weisman, C. (2021). Non‐financial conflict of interest in social intervention trials and systematic reviews: An analysis of the issues with case studies and proposals for management . Children and Youth Services Review , 120 , 105642. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moon, J. (1999). Reflection in learning and professional development . Kogan. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moore, B. (2005). Key issues in web‐based education in the human services: A review of the literature . Journal of Technology in Human Services , 23 , 11–28. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moss, B. R. , Dunkerly, M. , Price, B. , Sullivan, W. , Reynolds, M. , & Yates, B. (2007). Skills laboratories and the new social work degree: One small step towards best practice? Service users’ and carers’ perspectives . Social Work Education , 26 ( 7 ), 708–722. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Munford, R. , & Sanders, J. (2015). Understanding service engagement: Young people's experience of service use . Journal of Social Work 16 ( 3 ), 283–302. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Munro, E. (2011). The Munro review of child protection: Final report, a child‐centred system . https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/munro-review-of-child-protection-final-report-a-child-centred-system
  • Murphy, J. , Gray, C. M. , & Cox, S. (2007). Communication and dementia: how talking mats can help people with dementia to express themselves . Joseph Rowntree Foundation. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Narey, M. (2014). Making the education of social workers consistently effective: Report of Sir Martin Narey's independent review of the education of children's social workers . https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/287756/Making_the_education_of_social_workers_consistently_effective.pdf
  • Nerdrum, P. , & Høglend, P. (2003). Short and long‐term effects of training in empathic communication: trainee personality makes a difference . The Clinical Supervisor , 21 ( 2 ), 1–19. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nerdrum, P. , & Lundquist, K. (1995). Does participation in communication skills training increase student levels of communicated empathy? A controlled outcome study . Journal of Teaching in Social Work , 11 ( 1–2 ), 139–157. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nerdrum, P. (1997). Maintenance of the effect of training in communication skills: A controlled follow‐up study of level of communicated empathy . British Journal of Social Work , 27 ( 5 ), 705–722. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Page, M. J. , McKenzie, J. E. , Bossuyt, P. M. , Boutron, I. , Hoffmann, T. C. , Mulrow, C. D. , Shamseer, L. , Tetzlaff, J. M. , Akl, E. A. , Brennan, S. E. , Chou, R. , Glanville, J. , Grimshaw, J. M. , Hróbjartsson, A. , Lalu, M. M. , Li, T. , Loder, E. W. , Mayo‐Wilson, E. , McDonald, S. , … Moher, D. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews . BMJ , 372 , n71. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Papageorgiou, A. , Loke, Y. K. , & Fromage, M. (2017). Communication skills training for mental health professionals working with people with severe mental illness . Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews , 2017 ( 6 ), Art. No. CD010006. 10.1002/14651858.CD010006.pub2 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Parker, J. (2005). Developing perceptions of competence during practice learning . British Journal of Social Work , 36 ( 6 ), 1017–1036. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pedersen, R. (2009). Empirical research on empathy in medicine—A critical review . Patient Education and Counseling , 76 ( 3 ), 307–322. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Petracchi, H. E. , & Collins, K. S. (2006). Utilizing actors to simulate clients in social work student role plays . Journal of Teaching in Social Work , 26 ( 1‐2 ), 223–233. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Quinney, A. , & Parker, J. (2010). Monograph: The outcomes of social work education: Developing evaluation methods . Higher Education Academy, SWAP. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reith‐Hall, E. , & Montgomery, P. (2019). PROTOCOL: Communication skills training for improving the communicative abilities of student social workers—A systematic review . Campbell Systematic Reviews 15 ( 3 ), 1–9. 10.1002/cl2.1038 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reith‐Hall, E. (2020). Using creativity, co‐production and the common third in a communication skills module to identify and mend gaps between the stakeholders of social work education . International Journal of Social Pedagogy , 9 ( 3 ), 1–12. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reith‐Hall, E. (2022). The teaching and learning of communication skills for social work students: a realist synthesis protocol . Systematic Reviews , 11 ( 1 ), 266. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Robieux, L. , Karsenti, L. , Pocard, M. , & Flahault, C. (2018). Let's talk about empathy! Patient Education and Counseling , 101 , 59–66. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rogers, C. R. (1957). The necessary and sufficient conditions of therapeutic personality change . Journal of Consulting Psychology , 21 ( 2 ), 95–103. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rowland, A. , & McDonald, L. (2009). Evaluation of social work communication skills to allow people with aphasia to be part of the decision‐making process in healthcare . Social Work Education , 28 ( 2 ), 128–144. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schön, D. (1983). The reflective practitioner: How professionals think in action . Temple Smith. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schön, D. A. (1987). Educating the reflective practitioner: Toward a new design for teaching and learning in the profession . Jossey‐Bass. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Segal, E. A. , Gerdes, K. E. , Lietz, C. A. , Wagaman, M. A. , & Geiger, J. M. (2017). Assessing empathy . Columbia University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sidell, N. , & Smiley, D. (2008). Professional communication skills in social work . Allyn & Bacon/Pearson. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sinclair, S. , Beamer, K. , Hack, T. F. , McClement, S. , Raffin Bouchal, S. , Chochinov, H. M. , & Hagen, N. A. (2017). Sympathy, empathy, and compassion: A grounded theory study of palliative care patients’ understandings, experiences, and preferences . Palliative Medicine , 31 ( 5 ), 437–447. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Smith, J. (2002). Requirements for social work training . https://www.scie.org.uk/publications/guides/guide04/files/requirements-for-social-work-training.pdf
  • Social Care Institute for Excellence . (2000). Teaching and learning communication skills: An introduction to those new to higher education . https://www.scie.org.uk/publications/misc/rg03intro.pdf
  • Spreng*, R. N. , McKinnon*, M. C. , Mar, R. A. , & Levine, B. (2009). The Toronto Empathy Questionnaire: Scale development and initial validation of a factor‐analytic solution to multiple empathy measures . Journal of Personality Assessment , 91 ( 1 ), 62–71. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sterne, J. A. C. , Higgins, J. P. T. , Elbers, R. G. , Reeves, B. C. , & The Development Group for ROBINS‐I . (2016) Risk Of Bias In Non‐randomized Studies of Interventions (ROBINS‐I): Detailed guidance, updated 12 October 2016 . http://www.riskofbias.info
  • Sterne, J. A. , Hernán, M. A. , Reeves, B. C. , Savović, J. , Berkman, N. D. , Viswanathan, M. , Henry, D. , Altman, D. G. , Ansari, M. T. , Boutron, I. , Carpenter, J. R. , Chan, A. W. , Churchill, R. , Deeks, J. J. , Hróbjartsson, A. , Kirkham, J. , Jüni, P. , Loke, Y. K. , Pigott, T. D. , … Higgins, J. P. (2016). ROBINS‐I: A tool for assessing risk of bias in non‐randomised studies of interventions . BMJ , 355 , i4919. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sterne, J. A. C. , Savović, J. , Page, M. J. , Elbers, R. G. , Blencowe, N. S. , Boutron, I. , Cates, C. J. , Cheng, H.‐Y. , Corbett, M. S. , Eldridge, S. M. , Emberson, J. R. , Hernán, M. A. , Hopewell, S. , Hróbjartsson, A. , Junqueira, D. R. , Jüni, P. , Kirkham, J. J. , Lasserson, T. , Li, T. , … Higgins, J. P. T. (2019). RoB 2: A revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials . BMJ , 366 , l4898. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tanner, D. (2019). ‘The love that dare not speak its name’: The role of compassion in social work practice . The British Journal of Social Work , bcz127 , 1688–1705. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tedam, P. (2020). Editorial . The Journal of Practice Teaching and Learning , 17 ( 2 ), 3–5. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Teding van Berkhout, E. , & Malouff, J. M. (2016). The efficacy of empathy training: A meta‐analysis of randomized controlled trials . Journal of Counseling Psychology , 63 ( 1 ), 32–41. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • The Campbell Collaboration . (2014). Campbell systematic reviews: Policies and guidelines (Campbell Policies and Guidelines Series No. 1).
  • Thompson, N. (2003). Communication and language: A handbook of theory and practice . Palgrave Macmillan. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tompsett, H. , Henderson, L. , Mathew Byrne, J. , Gaskell Mew, E. , & Tompsett, C. (2017). On the learning journey: What helps and hinders the development of social work students’ core pre‐placement skills? Social Work Education , 36 ( 1 ), 6–25. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tompsett, H. , Henderson, K. , Gaskell Mew, E. , Mathew Byrne, J. , & Tompsett, C. (2017). Self‐efficacy and outcomes: Validating a measure comparing social work students’ perceived and assessed ability in core pre‐placement skills . British Journal of Social Work , 47 ( 8 ), 2384–2405. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Toukmanian, S. G. , & Rennie, D. L. (1975). Microcounseling versus human relations training: Relative effectiveness with undergraduate trainees . Journal of Counseling Psychology , 22 ( 4 ), 345–352. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Trevithick, P. , Richards, S. , Ruch, G. , Moss, B. , Lines, L. , & Manor, O. (2004). Knowledge review: Learning and teaching communication skills on social work qualifying courses/training programmes . Policy Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Trevithick, P. (2012). Social work skills and knowledge: A practice handbook . Policy Press, Open University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Truax, C. B. , & Carkhuff, R. R. (1967). Toward effective counselling and psychotherapy: Training and practice . Aldine. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tryon, G. S. (1987). The Counselor Rating Form—Short Version: A factor analysis . Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development , 20 ( 3 ), 122–126. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Unrau, Y. A. , & Grinnell, R. M., Jr. (2005). The impact of social work research courses on research self‐efficacy for social work students . Social Work Education , 24 ( 6 ), 639–651. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Uttley, L. , & Montgomery, P. (2017). The influence of the team in conducting a systematic review . Systematic Reviews , 6 , 149. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vitali, S. (2011). The acquisition of professional social work competencies . Social Work Education , 30 ( 2 ), 236–246. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wilt, K. (2012). Simulation‐based learning in ethics education [Doctoral dissertation, Duquesne University].
  • Woodcock Ross, J. (2016). Specialist communication skills for social workers . Palgrave Macmillan. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wretman, C. J. , & Macy, R. J. (2016). Technology in social work education: A systematic review . Journal of Social Work Education , 52 ( 4 ), 409–421. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yoder, W. R. , Karyotaki, E. , Cristea, I.‐A. , van Duin, D. , & Cuijpers, P. (2019). Researcher allegiance in research on psychosocial interventions: Meta‐research study protocol and pilot study . BMJ Open , 9 ( 2 ), e024622. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yu, J. , & Kirk, M. (2009). Evaluation of empathy measurement tools in nursing: Systematic review . Journal of Advanced Nursing , 65 ( 9 ), 1790–1806. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zaleski, K. (2016). Empathy in social work . Contemporary Behavioral Health Care , 2 ( 1 ), 48–53. [ Google Scholar ]

8 Ways You Can Improve Your Communication Skills

Your guide to establishing better communication habits for success in the workplace.

Mary Sharp Emerson

  

A leader’s ability to communicate clearly and effectively with employees, within teams, and across the organization is one of the foundations of a successful business.

And in today’s complex and quickly evolving business environment, with hundreds of different communication tools, fully or partially remote teams, and even multicultural teams spanning multiple time zones, effective communication has never been more important — or more challenging.

Thus, the ability to communicate might be a manager’s most critical skill. 

The good news is that these skills can be learned and even mastered. 

These eight tips can help you maximize your communication skills for the success of your organization and your career.

1. Be clear and concise

Communication is primarily about word choice. And when it comes to word choice, less is more.

The key to powerful and persuasive communication — whether written or spoken — is clarity and, when possible, brevity. 

Before engaging in any form of communication, define your goals and your audience. 

Outlining carefully and explicitly what you want to convey and why will help ensure that you include all necessary information. It will also help you eliminate irrelevant details. 

Avoid unnecessary words and overly flowery language, which can distract from your message.

And while repetition may be necessary in some cases, be sure to use it carefully and sparingly. Repeating your message can ensure that your audience receives it, but too much repetition can cause them to tune you out entirely. 

2. Prepare ahead of time

Know what you are going to say and how you are going to say before you begin any type of communication.

However, being prepared means more than just practicing a presentation. 

Preparation also involves thinking about the entirety of the communication, from start to finish. Research the information you may need to support your message. Consider how you will respond to questions and criticisms. Try to anticipate the unexpected.

Before a performance review, for instance, prepare a list of concrete examples of your employee’s behavior to support your evaluation.

Before engaging in a salary or promotion negotiation, know exactly what you want. Be ready to discuss ranges and potential compromises; know what you are willing to accept and what you aren’t. And have on hand specific details to support your case, such as relevant salaries for your position and your location (but be sure that your research is based on publicly available information, not company gossip or anecdotal evidence). 

Before entering into any conversation, brainstorm potential questions, requests for additional information or clarification, and disagreements so you are ready to address them calmly and clearly.

3. Be mindful of nonverbal communication

Our facial expressions, gestures, and body language can, and often do, say more than our words. 

Nonverbal cues can have between 65 and 93 percent more impact than the spoken word. And we are more likely to believe the nonverbal signals over spoken words if the two are in disagreement. 

Leaders must be especially adept at reading nonverbal cues. 

Employees who may be unwilling to voice disagreements or concerns, for instance, may show their discomfort through crossed arms or an unwillingness to make eye contact. If you are aware of others’ body language, you may be able to adjust your communication tactics appropriately.

At the same time, leaders must also be able to control their own nonverbal communications. 

Your nonverbal cues must, at all times, support your message. At best, conflicting verbal and nonverbal communication can cause confusion. At worst, it can undermine your message and your team’s confidence in you, your organization, and even in themselves. 

4. Watch your tone

How you say something can be just as important as what you say. As with other nonverbal cues, your tone can add power and emphasis to your message, or it can undermine it entirely.

Tone can be an especially important factor in workplace disagreements and conflict. A well-chosen word with a positive connotation creates good will and trust. A poorly chosen word with unclear or negative connotations can quickly lead to misunderstanding. 

When speaking, tone includes volume, projection, and intonation as well as word choice. In real time, it can be challenging to control tone to ensure that it matches your intent. But being mindful of your tone will enable you to alter it appropriately if a communication seems to be going in the wrong direction.

Tone can be easier to control when writing. Be sure to read your communication once, even twice, while thinking about tone as well as message. You may even want to read it out loud or ask a trusted colleague to read it over, if doing so does not breach confidentiality. 

And when engaging in a heated dialogue over email or other written medium, don’t be too hasty in your replies. 

If at all possible, write out your response but then wait for a day or two to send it. In many cases, re-reading your message after your emotions have cooled allows you to moderate your tone in a way that is less likely to escalate the conflict.

Browse our Communication programs.

5. Practice active listening

Communication nearly always involves two or more individuals.

Therefore, listening is just as important as speaking when it comes to communicating successfully. But listening can be more challenging than we realize. 

In her blog post Mastering the Basics of Communication , communication expert Marjorie North notes that we only hear about half of what the other person says during any given conversation. 

The goal of active listening is to ensure that you hear not just the words the person is saying, but the entire message. Some tips for active listening include:

  • Giving the speaker your full and undivided attention
  • Clearing your mind of distractions, judgements, and counter-arguments. 
  • Avoiding the temptation to interrupt with your own thoughts.
  • Showing open, positive body language to keep your mind focused and to show the speaker that you are really listening
  • Rephrase or paraphrase what you’ve heard when making your reply
  • Ask open ended questions designed to elicit additional information

6. Build your emotional intelligence

Communication is built upon a foundation of emotional intelligence. Simply put, you cannot communicate effectively with others until you can assess and understand your own feelings. 

“If you’re aware of your own emotions and the behaviors they trigger, you can begin to manage these emotions and behaviors,” says Margaret Andrews in her post, How to Improve Your Emotional Intelligence .

Leaders with a high level of emotional intelligence will naturally find it easier to engage in active listening, maintain appropriate tone, and use positive body language, for example.  

Understanding and managing your own emotions is only part of emotional intelligence. The other part — equally important for effective communication — is empathy for others.

Empathizing with an employee can, for example, make a difficult conversation easier. 

You may still have to deliver bad news, but (actively) listening to their perspective and showing that you understand their feelings can go a long way toward smoothing hurt feelings or avoiding misunderstandings.

7. Develop a workplace communication strategy

Today’s workplace is a constant flow of information across a wide variety of formats. Every single communication must be understood in the context of that larger flow of information.

Even the most effective communicator may find it difficult to get their message across without a workplace communication strategy.

A communication strategy is the framework within which your business conveys and receives information. It can — and should — outline how and what you communicate to customers and clients, stakeholders, and managers and employees. 

Starting most broadly, your strategy should incorporate who gets what message and when. This ensures that everyone receives the correct information at the right time. 

It can be as detailed as how you communicate, including defining the type of tools you use for which information. For example, you may define when it’s appropriate to use a group chat for the entire team or organization or when a meeting should have been summarized in an email instead. 

Creating basic guidelines like this can streamline the flow of information. It will help ensure that everyone gets the details they need and that important knowledge isn’t overwhelmed by extraneous minutia. 

8. Create a positive organizational culture

The corporate culture in which you are communicating also plays a vital role in effective communication. 

In a positive work environment — one founded on transparency, trust, empathy, and open dialogue — communication in general will be easier and more effective. 

Employees will be more receptive to hearing their manager’s message if they trust that manager. And managers will find it easier to create buy-in and even offer constructive criticism if they encourage their employees to speak up, offer suggestions, and even offer constructive criticisms of their own. 

“The most dangerous organization is a silent one,” says Lorne Rubis in a blog post, Six Tips for Building a Better Workplace Culture . Communication, in both directions, can only be effective in a culture that is built on trust and a foundation of psychological safety.

Authoritative managers who refuse to share information, aren’t open to suggestions, and refuse to admit mistakes and accept criticism are likely to find their suggestions and criticisms met with defensiveness or even ignored altogether. 

Without that foundation of trust and transparency, even the smallest communication can be misconstrued and lead to misunderstandings and unnecessary conflict.

Communicating with co-workers and employees is always going to present challenges. There will always be misunderstandings and miscommunications that must be resolved and unfortunately, corporate messages aren’t always what we want to hear, especially during difficult times.

But building and mastering effective communication skills will make your job easier as a leader, even during difficult conversations. Taking the time to build these skills will certainly be time well-spent. 

Want to build your skills? Find the program that’s right for you.

Browse all Professional & Executive Development programs.

About the Author

Digital Content Producer

Emerson is a Digital Content Producer at Harvard DCE. She is a graduate of Brandeis University and Yale University and started her career as an international affairs analyst. She is an avid triathlete and has completed three Ironman triathlons, as well as the Boston Marathon.

Harvard Professional Development Participant Success Stories

Read about how these skilled professionals used the knowledge and skills they learned in a Harvard PDP to further their career development.

Harvard Division of Continuing Education

The Division of Continuing Education (DCE) at Harvard University is dedicated to bringing rigorous academics and innovative teaching capabilities to those seeking to improve their lives through education. We make Harvard education accessible to lifelong learners from high school to retirement.

Harvard Division of Continuing Education Logo

Educational resources and simple solutions for your research journey

communicating research

The Importance of Communication in Research

Researchers spend a significant amount of time and resources conducting research studies in the hope of generating new knowledge and insights. However, if they are unable to effectively communicate their findings to relevant audiences, their work could go unnoticed. Therefore, the importance of communication in research cannot be emphasised enough.

Table of Contents

Why is communicating research important in research?

There are several reasons why you must communicate the research you write. Let’s read why communicating research is important.

  • It helps boost awareness of your work: Communicating research findings to wider audiences can help bridge the gap between academic research and public understanding and goes a long way in boosting researcher credibility. When the public is informed about your research, it increases the chances of them supporting the research financially and participating in studies or clinical trials.
  • It helps you get the funding you need: Effective research communication can help you convince funding agencies that your research is worth investing in. A well-presented research proposal can help funders understand the significance of your research, the potential impact it could have, and the methods you plan to use to achieve your research goals.
  • It helps peers and colleagues to make informed and ethical decisions: Scientific research plays a significant role in informing policy decisions, but if research findings are not communicated effectively, it can lead to misunderstandings, poor decision-making, and even harm. Therefore, communicating research findings in a clear, concise, and transparent manner is important so that your peers can understand your research and use it appropriately.
  • It can potentially fuel discoveries that advance science: Effective research communication can help researchers to build networks, collaborate with others and get access to the latest research findings, which can lead to discoveries that further advance our understanding of science.

communication skills research

Why is it difficult to communicate research effectively?

There are several reasons why people find it difficult to communicate research effectively. Some research studies, for example, may be complex and involve multiple variables or methods. It can be challenging to explain these complex concepts in a way that is easily understandable for non-experts. Additionally, many researchers struggle with translating their research findings into English as they do not speak the language. This makes it difficult for the larger scientific community and other interested audiences to understand the significance of their research.

Researchers must manage the challenge of tailoring their communication style to be able to attract and engage with a wide range of audiences, including policymakers, funders, and the general public. This is very important as ineffective research communication can lead to misinterpretation and misunderstandings of the research findings, which can have negative consequences. For example, if policy decisions are based on incomplete or inaccurate information, it could lead to unintended consequences.

Tips for communicating research to a broader audience

Striking a balance between an easy-to-read paper and a technically sound one is a challenging task. Here are some practical tips that researchers can employ in communicating research accurately and effectively without diluting it.

  • Know your audience: Researchers should tailor their communication to the audience they are addressing, whether it be policymakers, other researchers, or the general public. If, for example, you are addressing the scientific community, you may want to lean more on the technical side. On the other hand, if you are writing for broader audiences you may want to use analogies from everyday life to explain your findings.
  • Use simple language: While it is important for researchers to ensure that their manuscripts are accurate and technically sound, using jargon and technically complex language can limit readability. For example, instead of using the word ‘Hydrophilic’ you can use the term ‘ Hydrogen loving’ which is easier to understand. ‘De jure’ can be simplified to ‘ according to law’ . This makes it easier for a layman to understand your work.
  • Focus on the important parts: As a researcher, it is easy to fall into the trap of explaining all the details of your experiment to readers. However, the reader does not need to know every technical detail. Communicating research should focus on how the results and inferences impact our understanding of things. While the methodology behind the study can be shared, getting into minute details will cause a loss of attention and consequently loss of readership.
  • Make use of metaphors to relay your point: Sometimes, technical concepts are complex and explanations are not easy to convey. Here, using analogies or metaphors with something simpler can prove invaluable. For example, instead of explaining a pendulum and what it does, we can liken it to a swing used by a kid. This helps communicate research in an easy-to-understand way.  
  • Add visual representation to make things engaging: Text accompanied with images and graphs are a great way to attract and hold reader attention. For example, if we are trying to highlight the share of different nations in the world economically, using a graph can help convey data quickly and accurately.
  • Ensure transparency and accuracy in your writing: It is essential to be transparent and honest when communicating research findings. This means acknowledging limitations, caveats, and uncertainties, and being clear about what the research findings do and do not show.

Remember, while most researchers find writing to be a daunting task, communicating research is a skill that can be mastered like any other skill. All it requires is patience, practice, and perseverance.

Editage All Access is a subscription-based platform that unifies the best AI tools and services designed to speed up, simplify, and streamline every step of a researcher’s journey. The Editage All Access Pack is a one-of-a-kind subscription that unlocks full access to an AI writing assistant, literature recommender, journal finder, scientific illustration tool, and exclusive discounts on professional publication services from Editage.  

Based on 22+ years of experience in academia, Editage All Access empowers researchers to put their best research forward and move closer to success. Explore our top AI Tools pack, AI Tools + Publication Services pack, or Build Your Own Plan. Find everything a researcher needs to succeed, all in one place –  Get All Access now starting at just $14 a month !    

Related Posts

Back to school 2024 sale

Back to School – Lock-in All Access Pack for a Year at the Best Price

journal turnaround time

Journal Turnaround Time: Researcher.Life and Scholarly Intelligence Join Hands to Empower Researchers with Publication Time Insights 

  • Business Essentials
  • Leadership & Management
  • Credential of Leadership, Impact, and Management in Business (CLIMB)
  • Entrepreneurship & Innovation
  • Digital Transformation
  • Finance & Accounting
  • Business in Society
  • For Organizations
  • Support Portal
  • Media Coverage
  • Founding Donors
  • Leadership Team

communication skills research

  • Harvard Business School →
  • HBS Online →
  • Business Insights →

Business Insights

Harvard Business School Online's Business Insights Blog provides the career insights you need to achieve your goals and gain confidence in your business skills.

  • Career Development
  • Communication
  • Decision-Making
  • Earning Your MBA
  • Negotiation
  • News & Events
  • Productivity
  • Staff Spotlight
  • Student Profiles
  • Work-Life Balance
  • AI Essentials for Business
  • Alternative Investments
  • Business Analytics
  • Business Strategy
  • Business and Climate Change
  • Creating Brand Value
  • Design Thinking and Innovation
  • Digital Marketing Strategy
  • Disruptive Strategy
  • Economics for Managers
  • Entrepreneurship Essentials
  • Financial Accounting
  • Global Business
  • Launching Tech Ventures
  • Leadership Principles
  • Leadership, Ethics, and Corporate Accountability
  • Leading Change and Organizational Renewal
  • Leading with Finance
  • Management Essentials
  • Negotiation Mastery
  • Organizational Leadership
  • Power and Influence for Positive Impact
  • Strategy Execution
  • Sustainable Business Strategy
  • Sustainable Investing
  • Winning with Digital Platforms

8 Essential Leadership Communication Skills

Businessman leading team during meeting

  • 14 Nov 2019

If you want to be an effective leader , you need to excel in communication. In fact, the success of your business relies on it.

According to a report from the Economist Intelligence Unit (pdf) , poor communication can lead to low morale, missed performance goals, and even lost sales. A separate study found that inadequate communication can cost large companies an average of $64.2 million per year, while smaller organizations are at risk of losing $420,000 annually.

But effective communication impacts more than just the bottom line. For leaders, it’s what enables them to rally their team around a shared vision, empower employees , build trust, and successfully navigate organizational change .

Why Is Communication Important in Leadership?

A leader is someone who inspires positive, incremental change by empowering those around them to work toward common objectives. A leader’s most powerful tool for doing so is communication.

Effective communication is vital to gain trust, align efforts in the pursuit of goals, and inspire positive change. When communication is lacking, important information can be misinterpreted, causing relationships to suffer and, ultimately, creating barriers that hinder progress.

If you’re interested in enhancing your leadership capabilities, here are eight communication skills you need to be more effective in your role.

How to Become a More Effective Leader | Access Your Free E-Book | Download Now

Essential Communication Skills for Leaders

1. ability to adapt your communication style.

Different communication styles are the most frequently cited cause of poor communication, according to the Economist Intelligence Unit (pdf) , and can lead to more significant issues, such as unclear priorities and increased stress.

It’s essential to identify your leadership style , so that you can better understand how you’re interacting with, and perceived by, employees across the organization. For example, if you’re an authoritative leader , you likely have a clear vision for achieving success and align your team accordingly. While an effective approach for some, it might fall flat for others who seek more autonomy in their role.

Every employee’s motivations are different, so knowing how to tailor your communication is essential to influencing others and reaching organizational goals.

Related: 4 Tips for Developing Your Personal Leadership Style

2. Active Listening

Effective leaders know when they need to talk and, more importantly, when they need to listen. Show that you care by asking for employees’ opinions, ideas, and feedback. And when they do share, actively engage in the conversation—pose questions, invite them to elaborate, and take notes.

It’s important to stay in the moment and avoid interrupting. Keep your focus on the employee and what it is they’re saying. To achieve that, you also need to eliminate any distractions, including constant pings on your cell phone or checking incoming emails.

3. Transparency

In a survey by the American Management Association , more than a third of senior managers, executives, and employees said they “hardly ever” know what’s going on in their organizations. Transparency can go a long way in breaking down that communication barrier.

By speaking openly about the company’s goals, opportunities, and challenges, leaders can build trust amongst their team and foster an environment where employees feel empowered to share their ideas and collaborate. Just acknowledging mistakes can encourage experimentation and create a safe space for active problem-solving.

Every individual should understand the role they play in the company’s success. The more transparent leaders are, the easier it is for employees to make that connection.

When communicating with employees, speak in specifics. Define the desired result of a project or strategic initiative and be clear about what you want to see achieved by the end of each milestone. If goals aren’t being met, try simplifying your message further or ask how you can provide additional clarity or help.

The more clear you are, the less confusion there will be around priorities. Employees will know what they’re working toward and feel more engaged in the process.

5. Ability to Ask Open-Ended Questions

If you want to understand employees’ motivations, thoughts, and goals better, practice asking open-ended questions. Jennifer Currence, president of consulting firm The Currence Group, said to the Society of Human Resource Management to use the acronym TED, which stands for:

  • “ T ell me more.”
  • “ E xplain what you mean.”
  • “ D efine that term or concept for me.”

By leveraging those phrases when speaking with your team, you can elicit more thoughtful, thorough responses and ensure you also have clarity around what they need from you to succeed.

There’s a reason empathy has been ranked the top leadership skill needed for success . The better you get at acknowledging and understanding employees’ feelings and experiences, the more heard and valued they’ll feel.

In a recent survey (pdf) , 96 percent of respondents said it was important for their employers to demonstrate empathy, yet 92 percent claimed it remains undervalued. If you want to improve your communication and build a stronger, more productive culture, practice responding with empathy.

Related: Emotional Intelligence Skills: What They Are & How to Develop Them

7. Open Body Language

Communication isn’t just what you say; it’s how you carry yourself. Ninety-three percent of communication’s impact comes from nonverbal cues, according to executive coach Darlene Price .

To ensure you’re conveying the right message, focus on your body language. If you’re trying to inspire someone, talking with clenched fists and a furrowed brow isn’t going to send the right message. Instead, make eye contact to establish interest and rapport and flash a genuine smile to convey warmth and trust.

8. Receiving and Implementing Feedback

Asking for feedback from your team can not only help you grow as a leader, but build trust among your colleagues. It’s critical, though, that you don’t just listen to the feedback. You also need to act on it.

If you continue to receive feedback from your team, but don’t implement any changes, they’re going to lose faith in your ability to follow through. It’s likely there will be comments you can’t immediately act on—be transparent about that. By letting your employees know they were heard and then apprising them of any progress you can, or do, make, they’ll feel as though you value their perspective and are serious about improving.

Related: How to Give Feedback Effectively

Leadership Principles | Unlock your leadership potential | Learn More

Improving Your Leadership Communication

Communication is at the core of effective leadership. If you want to influence and inspire your team, you need to practice empathy and transparency, and understand how others perceive you, through your verbal and non-verbal cues.

To improve your communication skills and become a better leader, begin by assessing your effectiveness so you can identify areas for improvement. Then, set goals and hold yourself accountable by creating a leadership development plan to guide and track your progress.

Do you want to enhance your leadership skills? Download our free leadership e-book and explore our online course Leadership Principles to discover how you can become a more effective leader and unleash the potential in yourself and others.

(This post was updated on June 16, 2020. It was originally published on November 14, 2019.)

communication skills research

About the Author

INLP CENTER - NLP AND LIFE COACH TRAINING

The Importance of Communication Skills [Top 10 Studies]

Importance of communication skills – get inspired.

Never underestimate the importance of communication skills. This post reviews 10 scientific studies in varied areas of life. Each study underscores how important communication skills are and highlights the surprising benefits of using communication skills effectively.

My neighbor’s son just started his first year of college with a required course: Interpersonal Communication Skills 101.

“Yeah, I’m sure it’s some b.s. class, but I have to take it, so…..whatever. What does communication skills even mean?” he remarked.

And that’s a common view of the importance of communication skills. What student thinks it’s important enough to take seriously? I felt the same way when I was younger, until I read How to Win Friends and Influence People . Since then, I’ve never questioned the value of good communication.

In modern corporations, communication skills are considered “soft science” which is not far removed from “pseudoscientific waste of time” given the lackluster attitude with which many employees approach the topic.

To be fair, a lot of soft skills education is less than inspiring. When you consider the actual importance communication skills as evidenced by the wildly convincing studies below, you’d think mainstream educators would move beyond active listening and ‘I messages’.

As a life coach training and NLP certification school, we’re raising the bar on communication skills education. Let’s make effective communication more than an emoji! Here are 10 solid reasons the underscore the importance of communication skills :

The Importance of Communication Skills Demonstrated Through 10 Studies

1. better communicators have better marriages.

importance of communication skills

According to a recent study, the number one cause for divorce is communication issues. John Gottman, who has conducted studies on relationships for over forty years at the University of Washington, came to the conclusion that the thing people struggle with the very most is effectively communicating with one another in a relationship .

This validated information suggests that those who CAN effectively communicate in a relationship see more success and likely have a stronger connection to their spouse , which can be defined as having an overall better marriage.

His research suggests that there are four types of communication problems that can lead to divorce: criticism of partners’ personality, contempt, defensiveness, and stonewalling (the refusal to communicate at all). The importance of communication skills is essential in a healthy relationship.

References: Poor Communication Is The #1 Reason Couples Split Up: Survey | Huffington Post. (n.d.). Retrieved from http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2013/11/20/divorce-causes-_n_4304466.html Research FAQs – The Gottman Institute. (n.d.). Retrieved from https://www.gottman.com/about/research/faq/

2. Highly skilled communicators make more money

importance of communication sales

According to a survey conducted in 1988 by D.B. Curtis and presented at the Annual Meeting of the Speech Communication Association says that the most valued skills in the “contemporary job market” are communication skills. The study surveyed 1,000 personnel managers and came to this conclusion.

This information suggests that the best skill we can invest in within ourselves is strong communication. If we can communicate extremely effectively, we will surely be able to land the job that will earn us the greatest amount of money.

References: Why Communication is Important: A Rationale for the Centrality of the Study of Communication. (n.d.). Retrieved from http://www.natcom.org/uploadedFiles/More_Scholarly_Resources/Chairs_Corner/Making_the_Case_for_and_Advancing_the_Discipline/PDF-ATD-JACA-Why_Communication_is_Important_%20Rationale_for_Centrality_of_the_Study_of_Communication.pdf

3. Good communicators have higher self-esteem

importance of communication skills for confidence

According to an article titled “Self-Esteem and Effective Communication Skills” published by Live Strong, studies suggest that people who are good communicators also tend to be mainly extroverted. The article says a 2001 study published in the “Journal of Research in Personality says,” extroverted people tend to have higher self-esteem. Extraversion can make it easier for people to approach strangers, talk in large groups and appear friendly. This can cause others to perceive extroverts more positively, potentially further boosting their self-esteem. People with low self-esteem may be anxious about talking to unfamiliar people and more uncomfortable in group settings.

References: Self-Esteem & Effective Communication Skills | LIVESTRONG.COM. (n.d.). Retrieved from http://www.livestrong.com/article/187227-self-esteem-effective-communication-skills/

4. Build A Successful Family Unit

importance of communication skills family

A study conducted by Pearson, J. C. & Sessler, C. J. in May of 1991 titled “Family communication and health: Maintaining marital satisfaction and quality of life” which was presented at the Annual Meeting of the International Communication Association in Chicago says communicating role expectations is related to family satisfaction. Supporting, disclosing, negotiating, positively distorting, communicating needs, and demonstrating the understanding of other family members are just a few of the communicative behaviors crucial in creating a family which is healthy and happy.

References: ERIC – Family Communication and Health: Maintaining Marital Satisfaction and Quality of Life., 1991-May-27. (n.d.). Retrieved from http://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED335722

5. Most Important Skill For People Entering The Workforce

importance of communication skills workforce

The International Journal of Business Communication published a study in which 354 managers were asked to rank incompetencies when hiring new college graduates. The study findings suggest that the most desirable quality in a new hire is effective communication skills. Unfortunately, it is also the number one incompetency on the list, followed by lack of problem-solving skills and self-motivation.

References: A Managerial Perspective: Oral Communication Competency Is Most Important for Business Students in the Workplace Jeanne D. Maes. (n.d.). Retrieved from http://job.sagepub.com/content/34/1/67.abstract

5 Soft Skills for an Executive Coach

6. Communication Is Among The Top Traits of Successful Entrepreneurs

importance of communication skills

A study published by the American Journal of Small Business, titled “Perception of Entrepreneurial Success Characteristics” asked small business owners and bank loan representatives about what they believed were the key factors in successful entrepreneurship. The top-ranked characteristics among these people were oral communication and listening.

References: Perception of Entrepreneurial Success Characteristics. (n.d.). Retrieved from http://csfstudy.com/uploads/Montagno_-_Perception_of_Entrenpreneurial_Success__1986_.pdf

7. Effective Communication Skills Aid In Development of Leadership Skills

importance of communication skills leadership

According to a paper presented at the Annual International Conference of the National Community College Chair Academy, Phoenix, AZ, The ability to communicate and accomplish goals, or the “voice” element, is taught through exercises developing both interpersonal and intergroup communication skills and utilizes mentoring and role models to help student development.

References: ERIC – Values, Vision, Voice, Virtue: The 4 “V” Model for Ethical Leadership Development., 1996-Feb. (n.d.). Retrieved from http://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED394542

8. Helps People to Become More Critical of the Media

importance of communication skills media

This study suggests that education of communication in the media can help people to stop and consider the sources, and evaluate the visual artistic messages being portrayed by the media source. According to the study findings, the second section [of this paper] explains how understanding society and institutions will help the individual viewer to create the necessary standards for the recognition and evaluation of moving images, particularly television images. The final section stresses the importance of the viewers’ knowledge of the technical and artistic aspects of any given visual communication medium in the creation of the criteria for evaluating its messages.

References: ERIC – Cognitive Factors in the Study of Visual Images: Moving Image Recognition Standards., 1992-Oct. (n.d.). Retrieved from http://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED352936

9. The Communicatory Ability to Speak Gives You the Tools to Participate in Society

importance of communication skills public speaking

Students should learn to see reading and writing as vital support for the most direct way that citizens can express themselves and participate in public life—as public speakers. Public speaking was the primary medium for participation in public affairs at the birth of democracy in ancient Athens, and even today public dialogue or argument is, for most citizens, the chief means of participating in public life, according to a study, published by ERIC Digests titled “The Connections between Language Education and Civic Education”.

References: The Connections between Language Education and Civic Education. ERIC Digest. (n.d.). Retrieved from http://www.ericdigests.org/1992-2/civic.htm

[content_chunk id=”12058″]

10. Good communication with parents determines child’s level of self-esteem, achievement, and better overall health

importance of communication skills parenting

An article titled “Parent-Child Communication Programs” published by Advocates for Youth says studies show that young people who feel a lack of parental warmth, love or care were more likely to report emotional distress, school problems, drug use and sexual risk behaviors [3,4]. Young people also report less depression and anxiety when using CBD and more self-reliance and self-esteem than other peers who discuss sex with their parents.

References: Schuster, M. M., Eastman, K. P., & Corona, R. P. Talking to Parents, Healthy Teens: A Worksite-based Program for Parents to Promote Adolescent Sexual Health. Public Health Research, Practice & Policy, 2006. Retrieved from http://www.advocatesforyouth.org/parent-child-communication-programs

All these studies demonstrate the importance of communication skills in a variety of situations. Communication skills can be learned in a number of ways. One very effective way is through the use of Neuro-linguistic Programming (NLP). NLP was developed to understand how we communicate and to develop intentional ways to be effective at communicating and connecting with others.

If you see the importance of communication skills and are interested in learning more about how NLP improves communication skills, please read this article:

Advanced Interpersonal Skills – A World Waiting to Be Born

How it works

Transform your enterprise with the scalable mindsets, skills, & behavior change that drive performance.

Explore how BetterUp connects to your core business systems.

We pair AI with the latest in human-centered coaching to drive powerful, lasting learning and behavior change.

Build leaders that accelerate team performance and engagement.

Unlock performance potential at scale with AI-powered curated growth journeys.

Build resilience, well-being and agility to drive performance across your entire enterprise.

Transform your business, starting with your sales leaders.

Unlock business impact from the top with executive coaching.

Foster a culture of inclusion and belonging.

Accelerate the performance and potential of your agencies and employees.

See how innovative organizations use BetterUp to build a thriving workforce.

Discover how BetterUp measurably impacts key business outcomes for organizations like yours.

Daring Leadership Institute: a groundbreaking partnership that amplifies Brené Brown's empirically based, courage-building curriculum with BetterUp’s human transformation platform.

Brené Brown and Alexi Robichaux on Stage at Uplift

  • What is coaching?

Learn how 1:1 coaching works, who its for, and if it's right for you.

Accelerate your personal and professional growth with the expert guidance of a BetterUp Coach.

Types of Coaching

Navigate career transitions, accelerate your professional growth, and achieve your career goals with expert coaching.

Enhance your communication skills for better personal and professional relationships, with tailored coaching that focuses on your needs.

Find balance, resilience, and well-being in all areas of your life with holistic coaching designed to empower you.

Discover your perfect match : Take our 5-minute assessment and let us pair you with one of our top Coaches tailored just for you.

Find your coach

BetterUp coaching session happening

Research, expert insights, and resources to develop courageous leaders within your organization.

Best practices, research, and tools to fuel individual and business growth.

View on-demand BetterUp events and learn about upcoming live discussions.

The latest insights and ideas for building a high-performing workplace.

  • BetterUp Briefing

The online magazine that helps you understand tomorrow's workforce trends, today.

Innovative research featured in peer-reviewed journals, press, and more.

Founded in 2022 to deepen the understanding of the intersection of well-being, purpose, and performance

We're on a mission to help everyone live with clarity, purpose, and passion.

Join us and create impactful change.

Read the buzz about BetterUp.

Meet the leadership that's passionate about empowering your workforce.

Find your Coach

For Business

For Individuals

Request a demo

Foster strong communication skills to enjoy professional success

Find my Coach

Jump to section

What are communication skills?

The 5 main types of communication, why should you strengthen your communication skills, the top 7 communication skills for effective communication in the workplace, putting your communication skills to work, understanding yourself.

Humans are communicative animals. From early on, you meet your needs by expressing yourself via audible cries or nonverbal gestures.

Throughout your life, communication affects your fulfillment levels. You can tackle arguments quickly if you’re expressing yourself well and listening actively to the other person. And you’ll enjoy deeper relationships if you can share openly and be vulnerable . 

It’s no wonder workplaces prioritize strong communication skills when hiring. Employers want employees who express themselves well to ensure they’re being proactive about having their needs met and can handle workplace conflict tactfully. 

When looking for a new job , don’t underestimate the importance of strong communication skills. In a recent survey by GMAC, corporate recruiters ranked oral communication skills in first place on a list of 25 professional skills that included analysis, creativity , and drive.

Listening skills came in second, written communication was fourth, and presentation skills rounded out the top five.

In the same GMAC survey, 81% of recruiters said that interpersonal skills (which include communication skills) were most sought-after, and 57% said the demand for interpersonal skills will grow over the next five years.

It’s clear: developing strong communication skills is a wise investment in your professional future.

Communication skills are the abilities that allow you to effectively share your thoughts and emotions and understand others’. People with excellent communication skills express themselves clearly and effectively interpret what others say. 

In the workplace, effective communication is vital . If managers can’t communicate what they want or employees can’t succinctly describe a problem to coworkers, misunderstandings increase, as do mistakes and conflicts.

There are five main types of communication: written, oral (also called “spoken” or “verbal”), nonverbal, visual, and receptive.

1. Oral communication

This involves using spoken words to convey ideas. Politicians giving speeches, parents telling bedtime stories, and workers in Zoom meetings all use oral communication skills to express their thoughts and feelings and understand others’.

2. Nonverbal communication

Nonverbal communication is everything that’s unspoken when someone’s communicating with others, including:

Body language

Hand gestures

Eye contact

Tone of voice

Use of space

Appearance ( like clothing )

While nonverbal communication is most powerful in face-to-face contexts, such as in-person meetings and presentations , it also plays a role in remote work environments. Think about how closely you pay attention to people’s facial expressions (and Zoom backgrounds) in online meetings and webinars.

3. Written communication

Written communication involves using written or typed words (plus punctuation marks and emojis) to convey ideas. Unlike oral communication, written communication is typically asynchronous , meaning that writer and reader aren’t engaging with the message at the same time. 

Because written communication is often asynchronous, receivers don’t get as many supporting cues as in oral communication. You can’t see the author’s facial expressions or hear their voice’s tone.

If you’re not extra careful to convey the right emotional tone in written communication, you risk being misinterpreted.

Woman-writing-on-a-notebook-by-hand-strong-communications-skills

4. Visual communication

Visual communication involves expressing ideas and feelings through illustrations, images, and design. This type often supports other forms of communication. Infographics, for example, are a combination of visual and written communication.

5. Receptive communication

Receptive communication is the ability to decode oral, nonverbal, written, and visual communication. This is the half of the communication equation most people forget, but it’s crucial: what’s the point of writing a story or creating an infographic if nobody can understand it?

Having the right communication skills for a job makes you a more attractive hire. If they’re looking for someone to correspond directly with clients, for example, your written communication skills will stand out . 

But even after you get the job, communication challenges will surface in your professional life, like conflicts with a coworker or asking for a raise . To solve them, you’ll need to adapt your techniques and learn new ones constantly.

Strengthening different types of communication skills also helps you build and maintain working relationships , make more intelligent decisions , and motivate and inspire others . It could even increase your chances of getting a promotion since you’ll know how to communicate your value to your manager. 

Adding excellent communication skills to your skillset will also make you a better leader. Transformational leaders show exceptional communication skills .

They can explain complex concepts using simple language, use powerful metaphors to make their messages stick, and unite people by sharing the company’s mission.

Man-giving-university-level-presentation-at-school-strong-communications-skills

Here’s a list of the seven most crucial workplace communication skills with examples of communication competency in each and tips for developing them further.

1. Relationship building and maintenance

Human connection is fundamental to experiencing happiness, and genuine workplace connections improve one’s mental health . And the way you connect is by communicating with others: sharing your experiences, giving advice, listening attentively to show you care, etc. 

Tips: Small talk is only small if you do it wrong. To enjoy deeper connections, try empathizing with others , asking questions , and using the FORD acronym (family, occupation, recreation, dreams) to brainstorm topics. 

And to maintain these relationships, note small details about people (their childrens’ and pets’ names, things they said they were going to do, their food or drink preferences) and bring them up later. To repair relationships, learn how to apologize gracefully and sincerely. 

2. Group facilitating

Facilitation is guiding a group of people through a process to achieve a particular goal. Having to chair a meeting , resolve a conflict, or lead a group discussion are all nerve-wracking. But good facilitation is a skill you can learn — and the better you are at it, the less talking you need to do. 

Tips: Foster the right emotional environment to help the group reach its goals. When facilitating a brainstorming session, aim to generate positive emotions through praise and enthusiasm, as feeling good makes people more creative .

If you need to solve a problem together, channeling frustration can motivate people to find solutions.

3. Public speaking

Getting comfortable with public speaking will advance your career in leaps and bounds.

Public speaking helps you demonstrate leadership potential within your own company. It also builds your professional reputation, expands your professional network , and exposes you to cutting-edge advances in your industry through attendance at conferences and other professional events.

Tips: Even the best public speakers get nervous. Manage your anxiety before presentations by preparing thoroughly, breathing deeply , and practicing.

Consider asking for feedback on aspects of your nonverbal communication to make sure you radiate a natural confidence , warmth, and professional competence. 

Though some people recommend practicing in front of a mirror, this might make you self-conscious. Instead, ask a colleague or coach to listen, or even deliver the talk to your pet.

If it’s an online presentation, check the technology in advance, make sure your background is professional, and look directly into the camera. 

4. Storytelling

Storytelling is a popular form of written and verbal communication that’s especially effective for leaders . Research shows that leaders who tell great stories unite their workers under common values and even bring in more investment dollars . 

Tips: When telling a story, start with a hook to draw your audience in. Many workplace stories are about solving problems, so your hook could be a brief personal anecdote about a time you found yourself in a sticky situation. Listeners will want to pay attention to learn how you got out of it. 

To refine your storytelling skills, develop an elevator pitch: a compelling version of an idea or project that you can communicate in under a minute (i.e., if you’re in an elevator with someone, you have to convince them it’s a great idea before they arrive at their floor).

Woman-telling-a-story-to-a-friend-strong-communications-skills

5. Giving feedback

The best professional environments have a strong feedback culture . To create that culture, you need to be comfortable giving all kinds of feedback: constructive criticism that helps people improve, praise , upward feedback to your boss, and downward feedback to your direct reports.

Tips: When giving constructive feedback, show respect for the other person by listening to them carefully. Make it clear that you empathize with their position before making comments that may be hard to hear.

Consider whether structuring the feedback as a “ feedback sandwich ” (positive feedback, then suggestions for improvement, then more positive feedback) would help the worker assimilate the message. 

If you’re a manager, make sure you consistently show appreciation to your team , as this improves morale and performance.

6. Receiving feedback

Receiving feedback can be difficult, even if it’s positive. But knowing how to receive feedback well is just as important as knowing how to give it. Listening carefully to feedback helps you develop self-awareness, improve your performance , and take a more active role in your professional development .

Tips: When you receive difficult feedback, remember that the intent behind the words is to help you improve, even if hearing it makes you feel bad in the moment.

Try to regulate your emotions by finding commonalities between you and the other person or cultivating curiosity about the negative feelings you’re experiencing. If you receive confusing feedback, ask questions to clarify the steps you can take to improve.

7. Active listening

Active listening is an important factor of good communication.

When you listen actively, you’re not just sitting quietly and letting the words wash over you — you’re participating in the conversation by offering supportive verbal and nonverbal responses, asking questions, and paraphrasing what the other person says to show you understand.

Tips: To improve your active listening skills, eliminate all distractions and focus completely on the speaker. Ask open-ended questions to encourage your conversation partner to keep talking.

Avoid interrupting, but do use short verbal and nonverbal responses (“ backchannels ”) like “yeah,” “mm-hm,” and head nodding to show you’re listening.

When applying for jobs, show off your communication skills by incorporating storytelling into cover letters and relevant examples into your resume and LinkedIn summary , as well as using the STAR framework (situation, task, action, result) to tell focused stories in job interviews.

Show recruiters and future team members you’re a good listener by paying close attention to what they’re saying in interviews, rephrasing key points they make, and sending nonverbal signals that you appreciate their point of view.

When you receive a job offer, keep demonstrating your skills by writing a great offer acceptance email . Then enjoy exercising and developing your skills in the new position.

Woman-writing-an-email-at-a-cafe-strong-communications-skills

Building strong communication skills makes you a great hire, friend, and family member. But it also means you’ll better understand yourself. You’ll notice when your body language has become defensive and can ease back, or when your voice’s volume is too soft for a large room and can amplify.

In the end, you’re learning more about your behavior to exercise control over unwanted habits and live a more authentic life — and that’s priceless.

Understand Yourself Better:

Big 5 Personality Test

Allaya Cooks-Campbell

With over 15 years of content experience, Allaya Cooks Campbell has written for outlets such as ScaryMommy, HRzone, and HuffPost. She holds a B.A. in Psychology and is a certified yoga instructor as well as a certified Integrative Wellness & Life Coach. Allaya is passionate about whole-person wellness, yoga, and mental health.

7-38-55 rule of communication: How to use for negotiation

The 5 business communication skills worth perfecting, assertive communication skills: unlocking your confident voice, the significance of written communication in the workplace, improve your interpersonal communication skills with these 6 tips, how to end an email and leave a fantastic impression, discover how the johari window model sparks self-discovery, how to improve your listening skills for better communication, 15 human resources skills to help your resume stand out, 18 effective strategies to improve your communication skills, relationship-building skills examples to practice at work, 11 communication skills every leader should have, 10 essential workplace skills for success, how to identify and overcome communication barriers at work, stay connected with betterup, get our newsletter, event invites, plus product insights and research..

3100 E 5th Street, Suite 350 Austin, TX 78702

  • Platform Overview
  • Integrations
  • Powered by AI
  • BetterUp Lead™
  • BetterUp Manage™
  • BetterUp Care®
  • Sales Performance
  • Diversity & Inclusion
  • Case Studies
  • Why BetterUp?
  • About Coaching
  • Find your Coach
  • Career Coaching
  • Communication Coaching
  • Personal Coaching
  • News and Press
  • Leadership Team
  • Become a BetterUp Coach
  • BetterUp Labs
  • Center for Purpose & Performance
  • Leadership Training
  • Business Coaching
  • Contact Support
  • Contact Sales
  • Privacy Policy
  • Acceptable Use Policy
  • Trust & Security
  • Cookie Preferences

Improving Communication in Clinical Research

Improving Communication in Clinical Research

By: Anatoly Gorkun, MD, PhD, Chartered MCIPD             Senior Manager, Global Clinical Development, PPD UK

Abstract: Effective communication skills in clinical research are vitally important. Due to many conflicting priorities however, clinical research professionals may not have time to manage soft skills. This increases the danger that something may go wrong. This article highlights real-life clinical research examples where communication problems affected deliverables or compliance. The principles of effective communication styles are discussed.

Introduction

Communication is a key tool for clinical researchers, yet problems due to inactive communication are common. It is important to monitor possible ineffective communication in order to develop effective solutions to proactively prevent the negative consequences of ineffective communication.

Some time ago, the author received feedback from two clinical trial managers (CTM) on the same clinical research associate (CRA) at the same time. One clinical trial manager said:

(CTM’s Name) “is perfectly performing as expected from an experienced CRA. Her time has been allocated to manage a few difficult sites. Due to her learning agility and deep experience, the situation is improving now. She is a very good team player.”

The other clinical trial manager told the author:

“I know this CRA is new to the company and is still learning; however, with the upcoming data cleaning, I need your help.

I would suggest she has a co-monitoring visit with someone who is experienced. She needs to understand that this reconciliation is not just making a match between source data vs. case report form but also questioning what is being reported and identifying gaps, and being able to address issues with the site staff.

I would appreciate your feedback and actions.”

Both studies were relatively similar, and in this specific example, the problem appeared to be the communication between the CRA and CTM.

The Importance of Communication in Clinical Research

There are various definitions of communication, including:.

“The imparting or exchanging of information by speaking, writing, or using some other medium.” ( oxforddictionaries.com )

“Two-way process of reaching mutual understanding, in which participants not only exchange (encode-decode) information, news, ideas, and feelings but also create and share meaning.” (businessdictionary.com).

The second definition is broader and reflects the nature of communication more accurately.

Poor communication in clinical research has many negative effects (Table 1) including stress, possible conflicts between clinical research professionals, and a breakdown in relationships. Other negative effects of poor communication are unmet expectations (ineffectiveness), wasted time because work is inefficient and must be re-done, non-compliance, possible harm to subjects, and possible invalidation of data.

As an example: an in-house CRA approached the line manager and said, “I’ve done what the project team wanted, but when I finished the task they said it was not what they expected.” The line manager asked if the CRA had checked with the project team before starting the task to clarify what they wanted. She said, “No, because the task seemed very clear.”

Communication that is free of assumptions is one of the characteristics of ideal communication (Table 2). It is important to listen, ask questions to ensure understanding of the task, agree to what needs to be done, and confirm the agreement. Communication is a two-way process that requires mutual understanding.

Successful Communication Methods

The most suitable method of communication depends upon the situation and to some extent, the receiver’s preferences. For urgent situations, a telephone call is best, followed by an email to summarize the call. The communicator should not bombard the person with emails, because he/she does not know whether the person is receiving and reading the emails. In some situations, more than one communication method is appropriate, such as emailing instructions and then following up with a telephone call.

Sometimes it happens that the site monitor and the in-house CRA as well as the project assistant may ask the research nurse at the site the same question. This may not be the most efficient approach. Communication should be streamlined in order to prevent it from being chaotic.

The following email communication is between a clinical trial manager (CTM) and a CRA on an urgent issue that required immediate attention. A delay in resolving the issue might affect deliverables and the company’s image.

  • CTM → CRA, February 21: Check if all Adverse Events were entered into eCRF. Urgent, due in 2 days. Table attached.
  • CTM → CRA, February 24: “A kind reminder, please.”
  • CTM → CRA, February 24: “Please send me your answers today.”
  • CTM → CRA, February 28: “I need your answers, please.”
  • CTM → CRA, March 01: “Client requested us to provide the answer. Please complete this task.”
  • CTM → CRA, March 06: “I need your answers URGENTLY please.”
  • CTM → CRA, March 06: “Please do it tomorrow and let me know.”
  • CRA → CTM, March 06: “I would do, but I don’t know what to check.”
  • CTM → CRA, March 06: “The table is attached.”
  • CTM → CRA, March 08: “Any news from the sites?”
  • CRA → CTM, March 08: “Hopefully tomorrow.”

The clinical trial manager sent the first email to the CRA on February 21st and did not receive a reply from the CRA until March 6 th , nearly two weeks later. When the CRA responded that she did not know what to check, the CTM simply forwarded the same attachment. The CTM should have picked up the telephone and talked to the CRA.

Ideal communication is transparent. Transparency is:

“the perceived quality of intentionally shared information from a sender”

(Schnackenberg AK, Tomlinson EC. (March 2014). “Organizational transparency: a new perspective on managing trust in organization-stakeholder relationships,” Journal of Management . 10.1177/0149206314525202).

Transparency makes it easy for others to understand what actions have been completed and which actions need to be taken. It implies openness and accountability.

In another case, a project manager sent the following message to a line manager:

“As you know [name of CRA2] replaced [name of CRA1] at the end of March.

Unfortunately, by that date, the site performance decreased with late queries and SDV (Source  Data Verification) backlog due to pending monitoring visits.

Until today both sites still have not been visited and the plan is not available. I appreciate if you guarantee to have both sites visited by the end of April.”

The line manager spoke to the CRA, who said that the visits had been scheduled a long time ago. The project manager had been on holiday. When the project manager came back, he did not speak with the CRA about the status of the visits but instead escalated the issue.

Considerations in using appropriate communication include:

  • The purpose of the specific communication
  • How communicating will benefit the situation
  • Whether something different can be done
  • Whether alternative communication is necessary, and if so, the best method to use.

In the example, considering these four questions would have enabled the project manager to realize that talking directly to the CRA was the appropriate communication method for this situation.

In this case, the CRA needed advice from the CTM:

CRA: “I need to complete a number of overdue study-specific learning items on my LMS (Learning Management System) but I don’t have time. I am so busy.”

 CTM: “Then, do it wisely.”

The advice was not clear. Communication must also be concise. It is necessary to be clear about the purpose/goal of the message, to stick to the point, and to be brief.

Ideal communication is timely. In determining the best time for the message, the communicator must consider whether to communicate now or later. In some cases, it is better to wait and to communicate one message with another. It may also be helpful to pre-prepare the receiver of the communication with a brief heads-up.

A CRA was having communication issues with two clinical research sites. At the same time, this CRA had to deliver a presentation at a departmental meeting and wanted feedback from the line manager. The line manager knew that the sites were struggling to work with this CRA because of his insufficient communication skills. The line manager decided to wait a couple of days to speak with the CRA about his presentation/communication skills and the issue with the sites at the same time, as that was a good chance to demonstrate the importance of expressing thoughts clearly and explicitly.

It is also important to acknowledge receipt of an email or other communication when we are not going to provide our answer immediately. For example:

“I’ve received your message. It will take me a week to collect the requested information. I’ll get back to you by …”

Ideal communication is diplomatic and constructive. It is okay to disagree with someone; however, communication should focus on a person’s opinion or approach and not insult the person. For example, instead of saying,

“I don’t agree with you …,” say something like, “May I suggest that we discuss more options …”

An in-house CRA sent the following email message to a research nurse:

“I sent you my request 2 weeks ago, and it’s complete silence from your side. I find it so frustrating because we need to close all queries by the end of this week.”

The research nurse said that she would not respond to requests like this. After coaching the in-house CRA on communication methods, there was a visible improvement noticed, and the relationship with the site improved.

It is always better to be constructive. Avoid being very direct or pushy, and suggest options instead of criticizing or expressing frustration.

Ideal communication must be culturally respectful since clinical research is conducted internationally. Culturally respectful communication helps to avoid misunderstanding, to establish rapport, to build better relationships, and to facilitate more efficient work. Even among English-speaking countries, words or phrases can have slightly different meanings. For example, in the United States, “I hear what you say” means that the communicator accepts the other person’s point of view. In the United Kingdom, it may rather mean “I am not keen on discussing it further as I am not in agreement with this.” Also, accepting country or region-specific accents should be a part of cultural respect.

Ideal communication is also fair.

The following communication happened between a site monitor and a line manager.

Site monitor to line manager:

“I’m very busy and working very hard, however, I do not get enough support from the in-house CRA.”

The monitor’s line manager to in-house CRA’s line manager:

“I think that the in-house CRA might provide better support to the site monitor. Could you please check on the issue with the CRA’s performance and fix it?”

It turned out that the in-house CRA was doing a good job; however, the problem was that the site monitor needed to provide an explanation regarding the backlog of work that was created and decided to blame the in-house CRA for lack of support. Communication should be fair and should not blame other people unfairly.

Ideal communication is open, honest, and logical.

The following examples highlight communication between a line manager and a direct report during two performance reviews and a 1:1 meeting.

Mid-year performance review meeting, Line Manager to a direct report:

“You are leading a very important project really well. It’s going to be a great year for you!”

Monthly one-to-one meetings throughout the year:

The project delivery was on time and good quality. Every month, the line manager confirmed that she was happy with all of the work and there was nothing to improve.

End-of-year performance review meeting, Line Manager to a direct report:

“You’ve been struggling to deliver the project and managing it below expectations.”

The end-of-year performance review feedback was not logical because the previous messages were all positive. Ideal communication should avoid misunderstanding, conflicts, and disappointment.

Ideal communication is well-structured and compelling. Communicators should try not to tell a long story that makes it difficult for the receiver to determine what the communicator needs. This is important in everyday life with everyone, including communication with senior leaders and clinical investigators, both of whom are usually very busy.

The “rhetorical kipper” from Gareth Bunn can be used to plan communication. Using this model, communication is designed from the “tail of the kipper” and delivered from the “head.” After presenting the topic, three ideas or points are presented, and then finally, the request is made or the main message is delivered. The author’s direct reports found it useful to apply the rhetorical kipper method. Proper communication should be positive, assertive, and inspirational.

Case Study on Different Feedback

The case study presented at the beginning of this article illustrated different feedback from two clinical trial managers on the same CRA. One clinical trials managers stated that:

[CRA’s Name] “is perfectly performing as expected from an experienced CRA.

She has been allocated to manage a few difficult sites. Due to her learning agility and deep experience, the situation is improving now. She is a very good team player.”

I would suggest that she has a co-monitoring visit with someone experienced. She needs to understand that this reconciliation is not just making a match between source data vs. case report form but also questioning what is being reported and identifying gaps, being able to address issues with the site staff. 

I would appreciate reviewing your feedback and actions.”

The reason for the different feedback was not different complexity of the studies but that the clinical trial manager was micro-managing the CRA. She had a different management style than the CRA was used to. The first clinical trial manager delegated the tasks and trusted the CRA to complete them. The second clinical trial manager required daily reports from the CRA, assuming that if there was no daily report, it meant that the job was not done. She also had negative experiences working with a previous CRA and assumed that the new CRA would act in the same way.

Thus, the second clinical trial manager’s style was based more on assumptions. After a root cause analysis of the situation, the CRA learned how to recognize different working styles and started working with the second CTM more efficiently.

Many skills are required for appropriate and effective communication (Table 3), including listening and observing, planning, and dealing with difficult situations dearly and empathetically. Methods of ensuring that communication is appropriate and effective include awareness of communication issues (Table 4). If we face a communication issue, we should not assume that it will disappear by itself. A root cause analysis should be done to determine the cause of the issue, and then a plan should be developed to manage the problem. The plan should include feedback to ensure that the other person understands and accepts the plan. Line managers can arrange for soft skills coaching and training for people who need to improve their communication skills. If nothing else works, such issues can be escalated.

In clinical research, it is important to monitor possible ineffective communication approaches and to proactively develop effective solutions to prevent negative consequences.

The Effects of Poor Communication in Clinical Research

  • Possible conflicts and breakdowns in relationships
  • Unmet expectations (ineffectiveness)
  • Waste of time (inefficiency)
  • Non-compliance
  • Possible harm to subjects
  • Possible invalidation of data

Ideal Communication

  • Assumption-free
  • Proper methods utilized
  • Transparent
  • Appropriate
  • Clear and concise
  • Diplomatic and constructive
  • Culturally respectful
  • Open, honest, and logical
  • Well-structured and compelling
  • Proper communication style

Communication Skills

  • Listening and observing
  • Nonverbal communication (body language, facial countenance)
  • Negotiations
  • Dealing with difficult situations
  • Friendliness
  • Flexibility (open-mind)
  • Giving and receiving feedback

Communication Strategies to Prevent or Fix Issues

  • Be aware of communication issues
  • Observe and discover issues
  • Do not assume that the issues disappear by themselves
  • Perform root-cause analysis 
  • Feedback, and how to ensure that it is understood and accepted
  • Soft skills coaching
  • Soft skills training
  • Escalating (if nothing else works)

6 thoughts on “Improving Communication in Clinical Research”

Nice article.thanks

Be sure to communicate in the mindset of a team member, or stakeholder with the same goals and not as an outsider looking in with judgement.

This is a great inspiring article.

Nice article! Thanks for sharing this informative post. Keep posting!

  • Pingback: How To Succeed As A Clinical Trial Project Manager - Mosio

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed .

33RD ANNUAL CONFERENCE

ACHIEVING EXCELLENCE IN CLINICAL RESEARCH :

FORGING STRATEGIC COLLABORATIONS

September 27 to 29

COUNTDOWN TO ANNUAL CONFERENCE

Join us for expert-led sessions, interactive workshops, a peer-driven poster program, an engaging exhibit program and unparalleled networking opportunities!

communication skills research

  • About the Field
  • Generalist MCM
  • Marketing Communication
  • Strategic and Organizational Communication
  • Market Research and Analytics
  • Admissions Overview
  • Student Experience
  • Career Overview

MASTER OF COMMUNICATION MANAGEMENT ONLINE

Why Is Effective Communication Important to Career Success?

November 15, 2023

View all blog posts under Articles

Two co-workers shake hands during a meeting.

Effective professional communication is about conveying important information from one source to another. If that information is communicated clearly and effectively, businesses are more likely to run efficiently.

An advanced degree such as a Master of Communication Management (MCM) can help business leaders foster an environment driven by effective communication and a workforce that works toward common goals . While this skill can take effort to develop, the benefits are apparent across every industry.

Why Communication Skills Are Vital to Career Success

Nearly every job posting contains the words “strong communication skills” or “effective communication skills.” Good communication improves clarity in the exchange of concepts, knowledge, and ideas while reducing ambiguity or misunderstanding. This can affect businesses in a number of ways. For example, knowing how to communicate in the right manner to the right audience in a company can help create a more cohesive workforce. Communicating honestly and transparently can also foster a sense of trust and positivity, which increases work satisfaction and improves morale. Additionally, creating a culture of strong communication can help improve the exchange of ideas, potentially leading to increased creativity and innovation.

Effective communication is important to career success because leaders who build a culture of positive communication can help a business reach its goals with greater efficiency, produce satisfied workers and improve brand identity — all of which can translate to their own success.

Communicating With C-Level Professionals

In any position, a professional may find themselves in a position that requires communication with members of the C-suite. Although each C-level executive may have a preferred method of communication, knowing how to connect with someone in this role is vital to success. For some, the thought of presenting to or even chatting with the CEO, chief financial officer (CFO), chief operating officer (COO), chief information officer (CIO), chief technology officer (CTO) or chief marketing officer (CMO) is overwhelming. However, being able to effectively and comfortably communicate with these high-level executives can help individuals build trust in the C-suite, something that could lead to career advancement and success.

Learning effective communication skills can extend to communicating with C-level executives. Students in the online masters in Communication Management can gain the knowledge and tools to identify better communication strategies to apply to their audiences. For example, speaking to the CEO of a company should involve a different approach than speaking to an employee in that same company. A C-level professional likely has a lot less time to devote to a lengthy email, conversation or presentation, so conciseness is key.

Speaking the language of an executive is another facet of good communication that is important to business success. C-level professionals tend to focus on the bottom line, the potential for growth in the company, the metrics used to measure business progress and the goals, rather than the smaller picture. Speaking in terms of metrics and goals is a skill that can help a professional get the green light on a project they are working on when seeking approval from the executive team.

Mitigating Conflict

Good communication skills can also help professionals mitigate conflict in their careers. Every workplace will have some type of tension or conflict at some point, and these issues are nearly always traced to poor communication. If someone feels misunderstood, conflict tends to bubble up beneath the surface. In any type of management or leadership role, having the ability to communicate effectively, get to the root of the issue and resolve conflicts is extremely important.

Creating an environment that reduces conflict can have a positive impact that goes beyond internal issues. By nurturing an effective communication culture, leaders can help others cultivate communication skills that can be applied to diffuse potential external conflicts. For instance, a customer service team trained to deliver positive communication even in difficult situations can help de-escalate short-term conflicts. In the long term, this can help to build a stronger sense of customer loyalty.

Identifying and Building Specific Communication Expertise

Learning to communicate effectively is more than just knowing how to convey a message. Professionals must understand how to listen and respond to their audience, as well as communicate with respect in all situations. Cultivating these skills can help others be more receptive. In business, this can lead to being more accepting of new ideas that can in turn lead to innovative thinking and creative problem-solving.

In the online MCM program through the USC Annenberg School for Communication and Journalism, students have access to a world-class educational experience that can help them make their voices heard. This program applies data-driven strategies and real-world applications to help students gain experience in specific areas of communication. Students can learn how to balance the science and art of communication that can help them gain an edge in a competitive job market.

This higher education program helps students gain versatile skills like creative thinking and analytical processing, as well as the ability to convey a message in a manner that is effective and easy to understand. Those in the program may be looking to advance their careers, or they may be just starting out in the professional world and find it difficult to get ahead without effective communication skills.

Implementing a Communication Plan

Communication is more than just discussing day-to-day business processes. Communication should be incorporated into the company culture. When employees at a company feel like they can share their feelings and receive effective communication, they are more likely to enjoy their work and feel like valued team members.

Implementing an open-door policy is a good start, but leaders must take a more active role. They can start by delivering messages in a clear, concise manner while listening to feedback from the team. Doing so can create a sense of community and collaboration. The coursework and focus of the online MCM program can help an individual understand the importance of a communication strategy that impacts every department and employee.

Communication and the Bottom Line

Ultimately, stakeholders and business leaders must see the value of effective communication in order to implement strategies and policies that will improve it. Those who study communication in a higher education program can learn how it impacts the bottom line. For example, internal communication can impact the productivity of employees. When employees do not know where to turn for support or the resources they need to do their jobs, productivity suffers, resulting in a loss for the company. On the other hand, effective internal communication gives employees access to the tools needed, boosting productivity.

The company can also react more quickly and effectively to changes and shifts in the marketplace or industry when the leaders are aware of those changes and how they impact the business. As a result, a company can achieve or retain a position as a market leader when its executive team members stay aware of any major shifts in its industry.

The Importance of Communication for Remote Teams

Remote, work from home and telecommuting employees are becoming more common in many businesses across the globe. This was accelerated due to the coronavirus pandemic, as COVID-19 forced businesses to adapt to lockdown orders. Research indicates that remote work will continue to be popular in a post-pandemic world. Because of this, communication becomes even more vital for those who may be disconnected from the company’s physical office or other employees. Offering employees the opportunity to work remotely can lessen the financial burden of maintaining a brick-and-mortar location, as well as provide a beneficial and appealing work atmosphere for a potential employee. However, it is easy for remote and virtual employees to be forgotten team members.

Studying communications can help professionals understand how to better keep their virtual employees in the loop, feeling like part of the team. Digital and video communication methods have grown in popularity as remote workers have become more commonplace, and these methods are covered in the coursework of the online MCM program from the USC Annenberg School for Communication and Journalism.

The curriculum mirrors the interconnectivity of online and in-person communication strategies, connecting research to real-world settings for more hands-on learning opportunities. It is also a group-based learning atmosphere, helping students learn to communicate effectively with one another as they dive into areas of strategy and theory that they may not have explored in the past.

Develop the Skills That Lead to Success

The online MCM program from the USC Annenberg School for Communication and Journalism is designed to help students gain the skills they need to communicate more effectively and succeed in their chosen career paths. The benefits of the program include the opportunity to learn from faculty members with extensive experience in the communications industry and academia and the ability to connect with alumni across the globe. The program also ranks among the top in the nation in the QS World University Rankings.

The courses and real-life applications will explore current trends in communication, with a special emphasis on the importance of social media and digital communications. The program is meant to teach students how to think so that they can thrive in an analytics-driven and data-focused workplace. Businesses today need more than just strong communication plans. The leaders of successful companies need clear data that is driven by results and that shows how clients and employees perceive their businesses.

Theory is the foundation of the online MCM program, but it is not the destination. Through the study of a blend of theory and real-world situations, students can achieve a learning experience that goes beyond the textbook. They can study the objectives of communication, learning how to ask the right questions and what to do with the answers they receive. The importance of communication extends across every department, and students will take courses designed to help them learn how to speak and listen to managers, customers, stakeholders, the media and executives.

With an online MSC from the USC Annenberg School for Communication and Journalism, a professional may have more opportunities to design and implement effective strategies that will benefit every department of a business.

Recommended Readings

Crisis Communications: 2021 and Beyond

How Business Communication Rules Have Changed (And Stayed the Same)

13 Careers in Communication

Bizfluent, “The Importance of Communication in Customer Service”

Center for Management & Organization Effectiveness, “Team Leadership: Why Effective Team Communication Is So Important”

Entrepreneur , “How Effective Employee Communication Boosts Productivity”

Forbes , How to Promote Company Culture Through Your Internal Communications

Houston Chronicle , “Importance of Good Communication in Business”

Leadership Choice, “The Power of Good Communication in the Workplace”

McKinsey & Company, The Future of Work After COVID-19

Real Business, “Don’t Get Your Wires Crossed: Why Good Communication Is Key for Growing Businesses”

The Muse, “The Right (and Wrong) Reasons to Get a Master’s in Communications”

Get Program Details

Utah State University

Search Utah State University:

Effective communication skills: resolving conflicts .

Couple in conflict

Even the happiest of relationships experience conflicts and problems (Markman, Stanley, Blumberg, Jenkins & Whiteley, 2004). If handled well, issues provide opportunities for personal and relationship growth. There are many skills that can help individuals seeking to resolve conflicts in a healthy way. One of the greatest skills that aids in conflict resolution is effective communication.

Common Conflicts

Issues, or conflicts, in relationships consist of any situation, event or experience that is of concern or importance to those involved. A variety of factors lead to conflict, some of which include topics such as money, children, and in-laws, personal issues such as selfesteem, values, expectations, or goals, or relational issues such as the amount of together time versus alone time, support versus control, affection, and communication (Miller & Miller, 1997). While there are seemingly endless reasons for conflicts, they generally surround the underlying needs of all humans including physical, intellectual, emotional, social, and spiritual (Miller & Miller, 1997; Townsend, 2010). Most importantly, how we approach and communicate about these issues often determines the outcome.

Conflicts in Communication

Most people know that in order to resolve conflicts, we need to communicate about the issue; but negative patterns of communication can often lead to greater frustration and escalation of conflict. Consider the following communication challenges:

Body Language/Tone of Voice

Communication is more than the words we choose to use. In fact, our body language and tone of voice often speak louder than our words. For example, shouting “I’m not angry” is not a very convincing message! When we give an incongruent message where our tone of voice and body language does not match our message, confusion and frustration often follow (Gottman & DeClaire, 2001). In order to overcome this communication challenge, we need to be aware of what messages our body language and tone of voice may be sending others. Speak calmly, give eye contact, smile when appropriate, and maintain an open and relaxed posture (Paterson, 2000).

Differences in Style

Each of us has a unique way of communicating, often based on our family experiences, culture, gender and many other factors (Markman et al., 2004; Miller & Miller, 1997). For example, we may tend to be more loud, outgoing, or emotional when compared to our partner. While there is no right or wrong style, our past experiences often lead to expectations that are not usually verbally communicated with others, which can cause tension and misunderstandings in relationships. For example, if we came from a large family that tended to shout in order to be heard, we may think that speaking loudly is normal. But if our partner came from a calmer family environment, he/she may be uncomfortable or even frightened by a raised voice (Markman et al., 2004).

Discussing our backgrounds and perceptions can help to clarify expectations to ourselves and others and can also help our partner to understand our point of view. Knowing this information can often help in the problem solving process.

Communication Roadblocks

Communication roadblocks occur when two people talk in such a way that neither one feels understood. Research has found four particularly negative styles of communication, often referred to as the “four horsemen of the apocalypse,” (Gottman, 1999, p.27) because if left unchecked, these styles of interaction can eventually become lethal to relationships. These styles are criticism, contempt, defensiveness, and stonewalling (Gottman, 1999).

  • Criticism attacks the character or personality of another. While it is normal to have complaints about another’s specific actions, it is very different to put them down as a person because of those actions. For example, a complaint might be, “I felt worried when you did not call to tell me that you were going to be home late.” A criticism in the same situation would be expressed as “You are so inconsiderate, you never call me when you are going to be late.” Critiques focus on certain behaviors; criticism negatively focuses on the person’s intentions and character.
  • Contempt portrays disgust and a lack of respect for the other person through body language, such as eye rolling or sneering, or by name calling, sarcasm and cutting remarks.
  • Defensiveness is a seemingly understandable reaction that individuals take to criticism and contempt; however, it often escalates the conflict. When we are defensive, we tend to stop listening to the other’s viewpoint and communication is shut down.
  • Stonewalling is withdrawing from communication and refusing to engage in discussion. In other words, it is the adult version of the “silent treatment” that young children utilize when they are upset. Conflict resolution is impossible without communication!

Some additional examples of communication roadblocks include (Miller & Miller, 1997):

  • Ordering (“Stop complaining!”)
  • Warning (“If you do that, you’ll be sorry.”)
  • Preaching (“You shouldn’t act like that.”)
  • Advising (“Just wait a couple of years before deciding.”)
  • Lecturing (“If you do this now, you won’t grow up to be a responsible adult.”)
  • Agreeing, just to keep the peace (“I think you’re right.”)
  • Ridiculing (“OK, little baby.”)
  • Interpreting (“You don’t really believe that.”)
  • Sympathizing (“Don’t worry, it’ll all work out.”)
  • Questioning (“Who put that idea into your head?”)
  • Diverting (“Let’s talk about something more pleasant.”)

Communication roadblocks are very common; however, they do not promote healthy conflict resolution and often lead to escalation of the conflict. Recognizing these roadblocks and making efforts to effectively communicate can help individuals overcome roadblocks.

Tips to Resolve Conflict

Soften the startup.

One of the skills to overcome communication roadblocks includes a soft startup to the conversation by starting with something positive, expressing appreciation, focusing on problems one at a time and taking responsibility for thoughts and feelings (Gottman, 1999; Gottman & Declaire, 2001; Patterson, 2000). In addition, when expressing the problem, starting the message with “I” instead of “You” can decrease defensiveness and promote positive interactions with others (Darrington & Brower, 2012). For example, “I want to stay more involved in making decisions about money” rather than “You never include me in financial decisions.”

Make and Receive Repair Attempts.

Another important skill in overcoming communication roadblocks is learning to make and receive repair attempts (Gottman, 1999). Repair attempts are efforts to keep an increasingly negative interaction from going any further by taking a break or making efforts to calm the situation. This is important because when conflicts arise, we often experience intense emotional and physical stress that can impact our ability to think and reason, which can lead to communication roadblocks (Gottman & DeClaire, 2001). Taking time away from the conflict (at least 20 minutes) to calm down can help us be more prepared to discuss the issue (Gottman, 1999; Gottman & DeClaire, 2001; Markman et al, 2004).

Effective Speaking and Listening Skills

Overcoming communication roadblocks requires effective speaking and listening skills. Markman, Stanley and Blumberg (2010) share what they call the “speaker-listener” technique to help individuals more effectively communicate. Each partner takes turns being the speaker and the listener.    

The rules for the speaker include (Markman et al., 2004; Markman, Stanley & Blumberg, 2010):

  • The speaker should share his/her own thoughts, feelings and concerns—not what he/she thinks the listener’s concerns are.
  • Use “I” statements when speaking to accurately express thoughts and feelings.
  • Keep statements short, to ensure the listener does not get overwhelmed with information.
  • Stop after each short statement so that the listener can paraphrase, or repeat back in his/her own words, what was said to ensure he/she understands. If the paraphrase is not quite right, gently rephrase the statement again to help the listener understand.

The rules for the listener include:

  • Paraphrase what the speaker is saying. If unclear, ask for clarification. Continue until the speaker indicates the message was received correctly.
  • Don’t argue or give opinion about what the speaker says—wait to do this until you are the speaker, and then do so in a respectful manner.
  • While the speaker is talking, the listener should not talk or interrupt except to paraphrase after the speaker.

The speaker and listener should take turns in each role so that each has a chance to express his/her thoughts and feelings. Either can call for a time out at any time. The goal of this activity is not to solve a particular problem, but rather to have a safe and meaningful discussion and to understand each other’s point of view. While we may not always agree with the other’s point of view, understanding and validating other’s thoughts and feelings can improve relationships and help us build on common ground, which may lead to more effective negotiation and problem resolution (Gottman, 1999).

Dealing with conflict can take varying amounts of mental, emotional, and physical energy (Miller & Miller, 1997). It can be work! However, learning and implementing a few simple communication skills can increase positive interactions with others. The opportunities for personal and relationship growth are well worth the effort.

For more information or for classes and workshops:

  • Go to http://strongermarriage.org for tips, articles, and to find relationship education classes near you.
  • Check out your local Extension office for relationship education classes and events. 
  • Darrington, J., & Brower, N. (2012). Effective communication skills: “I” messages and beyond. Utah State University Extension. https://extension.usu.edu/htm/publications/publi cation=14541
  • Gottman, J. M., & DeClaire, J. (2001). The relationship cure: A 5 step guide to strengthening your marriage, family, and friendships. New York, NY: Three Rivers Press.
  • Gottman, J. M., & Silver, N. (1999). The seven principles for making marriage work. New York, NY: Three Rivers Press.
  • Markman, H. J., Stanley, S. M., & Blumberg, S. L. (2010). Fighting for your marriage. San Francisco: Jossey Bass.
  • Markman, H. J, Stanley, S. M., Blumberg, S. L., Jenkins, N. H., & Whiteley, C. (2004). 12 hours to a great marriage: A step-by-step guide for making love last. San Francisco: Jossey Bass.
  • Miller, S., & Miller, P. A. (1997). Core communication: Skills and processes. Evergreen, Co: Interpersonal Communication Programs, Inc.
  • Paterson, R. J. (2000). The assertiveness workbook: How to express your ideas and stand up for yourself at work and in relationships. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger, Inc.
  • Townsend, M. (2010). Starved stuff: Feeding the 7 basic needs of healthy relationships. Townsend Relationship Center.

Naomi Brower,  MFHD, CFLE, Extension Assistant Professor; Jana Darrington,  MS, Extension Assistant Professor

Naomi Brower

Naomi Brower

Extension Professor | Couple and Family Relationships | Weber County Director

Home and Community Department

Related Research

Building a Better Marriage

Building a Better Marriage

If you have been struggling with your marital relationship, or if you would like to improve the quality of your relationship, you are not alone. Just as Jenny and Michael want to strengthen their marria

Effective Communication Skills: Resolving Conflicts

Effective Communication Skills: Resolving Conflicts

Even the happiest of relationships experience conflicts and problems (Markman, Stanley, Blumberg, Jenkins & Whiteley, 2004). If handled well, issues provide opportunities for personal and relationship growth.

Effective Communication Skills: “I” Messages and Beyond

Effective Communication Skills: “I” Messages and Beyond

Communication is something we do on a regular basis. As young children we initially learn ways to communicate as we observe our parent, sibling, and family interactions. Throughout our lives our communication patterns evolve and are reinforced by our expe

Effects of Pornography on Relationships

Effects of Pornography on Relationships

Pornography is not a new issue in relationships; however, the expansion of the Internet appears to have increased pornography viewing and exacerbated pre-existing tendencies (Cooper, Boies, Maheu & Greenfield, 1999; Young, 2008). One key factor in this in

From Time to Quality Time: Making Every Moment Count

From Time to Quality Time: Making Every Moment Count

Couples and families often look for ways to find more time together and to make better use of that time. Most people struggle to find enough time in their day for everything. In fact, according to Dr. William Doherty (2001), those that care about each oth

Have Fun! The Importance of Play in Couple Relationships

Have Fun! The Importance of Play in Couple Relationships

Boring, drab, lifeless, stale, dull, tedious. These are probably not the words you hope to use to describe your relationships. How about well planned, frugal, precise, productive, serious, busy? Though these can be characteristics of a strong, healthy rel

Healthy Conflict Management

Healthy Conflict Management

Conflicts are a natural part of human interaction. Whenever two or more people are in the same environment for a long enough period of time, it is inevitable that conflict will occur. However, the conflict itself is not the problem, but rather how they ch

Honey, I’m Home: Strengthening Your Marriage Ten Minutes at a Time

Honey, I’m Home: Strengthening Your Marriage Ten Minutes at a Time

Strengthen your marriage relationship by making the first ten minutes of your interactions together a positive experience. Learn how to have stress-reducing conversation, emotionally support each other, and sooth self and partner in positive communication

Keys for Strong Commitment in Marriage

Keys for Strong Commitment in Marriage

Having commitment means being dedicated to a cause. Commitment comes in all different shapes and sizes, but the most important type of commitment, for many, is a commitment to your marriage. Often couples start their marriage with commitment, but they don

Making Media Work for Your Marriage

Making Media Work for Your Marriage

Increased options for instant connection can have positive and negative impacts on relationships. While online resources can help us stay connected to those we love and increase relationship satisfaction Pettigrew, 2009; Sidelinger, Avash, Godorhazy, & T

Making the Most of Marriage Therapy

Making the Most of Marriage Therapy

All relationships experience change over time (Larson, 2003). Even the strongest relationships can often benefit from a skilled a marriage counselor to help to smooth over the rough patches in their relationship. While the needs of relationships vary, som

Marriage Principles from a National Extension Model

Marriage Principles from a National Extension Model

Through this fact sheet, you will learn about these seven principles—Choose, Care for Self, Know, Care, Share, Manage, and Connect—and how to apply them in your life.

Supporting Others Coping with Infertility

Supporting Others Coping with Infertility

It is likely that you know an individual or couple who is impacted by infertility. The natural human response is to want to comfort them, but it can be difficult to know what to say or do, especially if you have not experienced infertility yourself.

Technology Tips and Traps in Your Relationship

Technology Tips and Traps in Your Relationship

This fact sheet will help you be aware of some of the positive and negative effects of technology and how to protect your marriage from being swamped by it.

Tips to Strengthen Relationships Today

Tips to Strengthen Relationships Today

Research conducted by Dr. Sonja Lyubomirsky shows that happier people tend to have larger circles of friends, experience strong social support, and are more likely to be a support for others. But this research also shows that the connection between happin

Science of People - Logo

10 Effective Ways You Can Improve Your Communication Skills

Want to become not only a powerful communicator, but an effective one? Here are the 10 most effective communication skills to help you improve!

Subscribe to our weekly newsletter

They say that communication is the most important factor in any work or romantic relationship… and they’re right!

Communication problems account for 65% of divorces in America.

But don’t fret.

What Are Communication Skills?

Communication skills help us share our ideas, thoughts, and feelings with others. Great communicators are able to give and receive different forms of information via verbal communication, nonverbal communication, and body language signs. Communication skills help us relay instructions, messages, new ideas, or emotions.

communication skills research

Why Are Communication Skills Important?

Whether it be verbal, nonverbal, or physical, we communicate with people every day!

Communication skills help us clearly relay our thoughts and ideas to others.

In 1952, Scott Cutlip introduced the 7 C’s of communication to help people create more sound points of interest while talking with another person.

These 7 C’s include

The 7 C's of communication is presented. It includes clarity, conciseness, concreteness, correctness, coherent, completeness, and courtesy.

  • Clarity : What is your purpose in communicating with this person?
  • Conciseness : Keep it short and stick to the point.
  • Concreteness : Create a vivid picture of what you’re speaking about with facts and imagery.
  • Correctness : Is what you’re saying error-free, and does it fit your audience’s level of comprehension?
  • Coherence : Your topic and points are easily understood and logical.
  • Completeness : Your audience has all the information that you spoke of and can act upon it.
  • Courtesy : Practice open, honest, and friendly conversation.

With the 7 C’s , you can ensure that your audience can listen easily to your words and employ them directly and peacefully. With that, you’ll become a credible and reliable source of information simply because you can communicate it for the masses to comprehend.

Also, keeping the conversation creative helps listeners to keep the spark alive and devise a colorful picture of what you’re saying in their head, which will keep them from finding excuses to leave the conversation.

Just know that the more you effectively communicate, the better you become at it and the more people will listen and adhere to what you’re saying!

Types of Communication Skills

By now, you’re probably wondering, What are some different types of communication skills that I can use to make people listen!?

We’ll briefly cover the key types of communication from a Drexel University study so that you get the main points:

Types of Communication which include visual, written, listening, verbal, and non-verbal communication.

  • Visual Communication : Physical or mental imagery that conveys your main points and explanations
  • Written Communication: Using various forms of written communication to inform in a clear and concise manner
  • Listening: Actively hearing words and phrases to connect points and comprehend the overall message
  • Verbal Communication: Speaking your words or message directly to your audience
  • Nonverbal Communication: Using your body to relay your point (e.g., facial expressions, pointing, eye contact, etc.)

These communication terms may seem familiar to you, but are you sure about how to use them?

If not, here’s how!

Visual Communication

Visual communication is said to be the easiest form of communication because our brains automatically create images of words and sounds that we hear. 

Between social media, ads, television, and our phones, our society has transformed into pictures with short and witty captions. These images convey a specific message that we want viewers to comprehend without thinking about it. 

As the saying goes, “A picture is worth a thousand words,” and that’s because of the various interpretations that we can conjure just by seeing some shapes, lines, and colors.

Written Communication

Remember being in school passing notes to your friends so that the teacher wouldn’t hear you? That was probably one of your first forms of written communication , and, as you can see, it works!

If you’re in the workforce, did you know that 73% of employers seek workers with top-notch written communication skills?

Why? Because memos, social media posts, and those pesky emails you send every day are a part of business and help us to effectively communicate by slowing down and expressing ourselves concisely without the hassle of being interrupted.

Also, think about it: How cool is it that written communication saves you from having to speak?!

This is a major help, especially for introverts , in getting your point across without having to say a single word.

Attentive listening tends to be one of the most important types of communication because, according to a listening study , a whopping 40% of communication involves listening!

40% of all communication happens through listening.

Without listening to the words that are spoken, you literally lose out on almost half of what was communicated; therefore, you can’t effectively engage in the conversation with the person speaking unless you’re hearing them.

Thankfully, there are four types of listening that we engage in daily.

Peruse the list of effective listening styles and determine which kind of listener you are:

  • Full Listening: Paying close attention to the words and tones of the speaker
  • Therapeutic Listening: Allowing close friends and loved ones to express themselves
  • Deep Listening: Focusing on learning the speaker’s core points and perspectives
  • Critical Listening: Using reasoning, facts, and logic to analyze a message

Luckily, these listening styles can be developed and enhanced with practice and consistent communication.

Verbal Communication

You can be speaking to a coworker via Zoom or hanging out with your friends discussing the latest in the celebrity world. Either way, you’re engaging in verbal communication .

Being verbal is a great form of communication that allows others to hear and connect the points you’re trying to make via your words.

Sure, some people, like Kanye West, are said to be “full of themselves” when they speak and could use a moment to simply zip it, but where’s the fun in that?

Like Kanye, many people use complexity and intonations (cadence, tone, pitch) to connect their surface words to their core message, which helps the audience understand their point.

Interestingly, while face to face, you can’t help but also take into consideration your audience’s nonverbal communication.

Speaking of which…

Nonverbal Communication

Have you ever received the “death stare” from someone? You knew exactly what that meant, and that, my friends, is a form of nonverbal communication .

Simply put, nonverbal communication consists of using any part of your body—except your voice—to get your audience’s attention and the results that you desire .

You’ve most likely engaged in these nonverbal cues before:

  • hand gestures
  • eye contact

According to a Darioly and Mast study , “Nonverbal plays an important role in interpersonal communication in general and accounts for a majority (about 65 to 90%) of the meaning conveyed in social interaction.”

However, a recent study made a valid point that if this is true, it’s because people aren’t paying attention to the words or are simply not listening.

This is exactly why knowing the different styles of nonverbal cues is necessary, and why nonverbal communication will never go out of style.

Top 10 Effective Communication Skills

To become a better communicator, you have to have the tools and skills to create the right message for the right audience, right?

Here are the top 10 communication skills that will enhance the way you speak and listen.

 1. Clear Signaling

“Communication—the human connection—is the key to personal and career success.” – Paul J. Meyer

In the realm of communication skills, clear signaling is the art of relaying your words directly and concisely to your listener .

It seems like an easy skill, doesn’t it?

However, it can be misconstrued based on interpretation, which leads to s ignal amplification bias .

Based on a study in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , signal amplification bias is also believing that a communication cue is worth more than its surface intention.

For example, many people aren’t sure others like them because they’re playing cool and undersignaling. In other words, many people are afraid others don’t like them. If you enjoy working with someone, tell them!

Signal amplification bias also stems from the misconception that you provided enough information to your listener when in fact you did not.

You can tell this bias is in effect when you hear statements like

  • “It should have been obvious.”
  • “This goes without saying.”
  • “I shouldn’t have to tell you this.”

Overcoming the bias requires initiating straightforward clear signaling. 

  • If you like working with someone, tell them.
  • If someone did a great job on a project, tell them.
  • If someone is making you uncomfortable, tell them!

Bottom line: Be honest with your intentions.

 2. Highlight Uniqueness

Uniqueness is the quality of being remarkable, special, or one-of-a-kind, and everyone has a special communicative trait that sets them apart from others. 

Whether it’s the inflection of your voice, an entertaining stature and body posture, or incredible clarity while speaking, you can improve your communication traits by highlighting your unique communication skills either verbally, physically, or nonverbally.

You can even show appreciation for others! Try highlighting other people’s uniqueness:

  • Compliment them on their unique style of clothing.
  • Send a message of appreciation about their orderliness or creativity.
  • Show appreciation for their specific personality traits .

3. Reading Faces

Reading faces is a vital communication skill that allows you to understand a person’s feelings through their face instead of their voice . In fact, humans have 7 universal microexpressions, or facial expressions, that signal different emotions.

The 7 different microexpressions are

In accordance, the facial feedback hypothesis states that one’s facial expressions are directly related to their emotional and behavioral experience toward others and themselves. 

A study was conducted to test this hypothesis with humor. The results show us that under the right conditions, people feel free to express their emotions through their face, which influences their emotional and physical demeanor.

In short, changing your facial expression can literally influence the way you think and feel—and the message you’re getting across!

This communication skill of reading faces is so powerful that facial reading training is available for people who want to increase their know-how of guessing how people feel based on their emotional facial expressions.

4. Still-Face Experiment

If you’re like Rihanna and have been consistently told that you have RBF, then you’ll want to pay attention to this communication skill.

Have you ever heard of the still-face experiment ? In a nutshell, it was a study done to show how stressful behaviors affect facial expressions and, thus, the emotional development of infants and children.

By the end of the experiment, it showcased that if you are still and emotionless in the face, it becomes challenging for people to communicate with you simply because they can’t tell if you’re paying attention or not.

If you have RBF or “Still Face,” there’s hope for you yet!

There are some solutions that you can use to gauge a deeper understanding of what is being communicated so that you can have some movement in those stale muscles.

Check them out:

  • Listen with empathy and verbally respond.
  • Help people to label their feelings so that you can physically empathize.
  • Recognize the speaker’s main point to connect on a deeper level.

You may get mad—heck, you may even chuckle at what’s being said—but the point is to stray from the Still Face and engage in facial reactions to show attentive listening and improved communication.

But if you’re REALLY struggling to fix that RBF, there may be hope. Check out our guide here: Resting Bitch Face: How to Fix Your RBF Forever (With Science) .

Let’s move on to the next skill.

5. Facial Absorption

Facial absorption is the face-to-face communication skill of taking in information and using your face to show your reaction . That’s it! 

Here’s the secret sauce to enacting this communication skill with other people: eye contact !

When you are focused on the person’s face while they are speaking, there is a chance that your mirror neurons will copy the speaker’s face and make you react in the same way. 

Facial absorption automatically shows coherent listening and courtesy, even if you don’t catch every single word that floods out of their mouth.

And the best part?

Facial absorption comes automatically. All you need to do is really pay attention to the other person during a conversation.

Here’s another tip: Use facial absorption during your next meeting, as it is one of the most impactful communication skills in the workplace .

6. Use Powerful Words

Words are very powerful, depending on how we say them and the specific words that we say.

In fact, using powerful words is one of the most valuable communication skills in the workplace because of the digital world we live in that simply requires us to look down and use our thumbs.

Social and corporate jargon can be a serious hindrance to effective communication, which is why words used should be powerful and easy to understand.

Here are some that you can try out:

  • transparent
  • challenging

A list of powerful words.

You also use powerful words to increase your influence toward others and reveal the same communicative traits in others that you have within yourself, which is referred to as spontaneous trait transference . 

We’ll put it this way: The way you describe others is the way people will see you —that’s the transference. And the fact that it happens instantly is what makes it spontaneous!

In the workplace, examples of this can be seen when your boss gives you a compliment or when you hear someone gossiping about another person.

They can be positive or negative, but the point is to use strong words to convey your message with positive assertion and tact and improve your oral communication skills .

7. Embodied Cognition

You know when you go to the doctor’s office for a checkup and you see them set up the tools to give you a shot? Your body starts to tense and your heart starts to race a bit, doesn’t it?

There’s a reason for that, and it’s called embodied cognition .

It’s the latest sexy topic in social psychology, theorized as behavior emerging from the real-time interaction between our nervous system and our environment, which persuades our mind to think a certain way.

So what does this have to do with communication?

Through strategic messaging, certain sounds, imagery, or voice inflections can make your body react, which may convince your mind to think a particular thought.

Other examples can be as simple as seeing a chair and thinking, I should sit , or, even if you aren’t hungry, smelling food and having the thought of eating pop into your mind.

Embodied cognition has been described as “ internal suggestive communication. ” It’s simply saying that the body can influence the mind as much as the mind can influence the body .

For example, there used to be a student who told her team, “Let’s not be a sinking ship” or “Let’s not rip the bandaid off,” until she noticed that her team members visibly winced when she said it. Even though she said NOT, the metaphor was still physically painful. Be careful with your words and err on the side of positive.

You can even hang up photos around your cubicle in the workplace and listen to audiobooks of your favorite public speakers to persuade your mind.

8. Sharing Feedback

Sharing feedback is responding to a message or activity .

No one likes to feel like they are wasting their time speaking, which is why sharing feedback is a HUGE communication skill that proves you paid attention.

It’s also one of the safest and most effective oral communication skills and business communication skills to create an engaging dialogue about the topic at hand.

A 2017 study gives a list of the different types of feedback . Check to see if you’ve engaged in any of them:

  • Informal Feedback : Basic verbal or nonverbal responses on performance or statements
  • Formal Feedback: A structured assessment where people give direct critique or criticism to the speaker
  • Summative Feedback: A detailed summary of the topic along with positive comments and solutions for enhancing shortcomings

Appropriate feedback is an important interpersonal skill because it contributes to development and confidence in receiving critique from people you know—and don’t know—while building your confidence in communication.

Action Step: Grab a “feedback buddy” and work on playing out imaginary scenarios, such as a job interview or giving a TED talk , while giving critiques and comments to each other.

9. Positive Body Language

Body language includes all communication through a physical channel and is a powerful form of communication—more powerful than words! 

Why? Because the body does not lie!

Social anthropologist Edward T. Hall (1959) maintained that there are more than 700,000 forms of body language , and during a typical conversation, 65% of social meanings are portrayed directly through body language .

Yet in different parts of the world, this number and body language will vary. 

Let’s break down what each body part could indicate while communicating (at least in Western cultures):

  • Head: A subtle nod can mean agreement, while shaking the head no (even if someone says yes) can mean disagreement. How do you spot these subtle differences? Learn more about head behavior .
  • Face: The face can give away subtle hints of anger, happiness, sadness, or even contempt! Cues can manifest in bared teeth or pursed lips. Learn more about facial expressions .
  • Eyes: Depending on where the eyes are looking, a person can be feeling intimate or bored! How do you tell? Learn more about the eyes here .
  • Mouth : Licking the lips draws attention through tongue movement. It also leaves the lips noticeably shinier and more attractive. What other mouth cues are there? Learn about mouth cues here .
  • Hands: Greetings, farewell, or threat. Hiding your hands can signal a threat. Open palms, on the other hand, signal sincerity. Learn all the hand gestures you need to know: 60 Hand Gestures You Should Be Using .
  • Legs/Feet: How do you know where someone wants to go? Simply look at their feet. Learn why in this article: 20 Leg Body Language Cues To Help You Analyze ANY Situation .

Learning how to improve communication skills by being sensitive to body language allows you to become aware of whether who you’re talking to is entertained or bored out of their spiritless mind.

It can help you spot contradictions between what is said and what is meant while also helping you to become more cognizant of your body language to determine what message you’re sending out to your audience.

And if you really want to master body language? Read our mega guide: The Ultimate Guide to Body Language .

10. Storytelling

Want to know how to improve communication skills at work , at home, or in society? Try the oldest form of communication that’s still used in many cultures around the world: storytelling ! 

Storytelling is a form of communication that creates colorful imagery backed by detailed words to both help your listeners understand your core message and keep them mentally entertained. 

It’s also a solid way to increase your verbal communication skills and one of the greatest active listening exercises for your audience.

Storytelling entails

  • using visual cues, such as pictures or objects
  • practicing rephrasing if someone doesn’t get your message
  • increasing nonverbal cues with bodily movement and gestures

If you want to learn more about how to improve communication skills through storytelling, read our amazing article: How to Tell a Great Story: Learn Science of Storytelling .

Communication Really IS Key!

Why are communication skills important? Because they’re what we use to survive. You can choose to wave a hand, make a face, or speak your mind. In any case, you have to use vital communication skills to effectively get your message across.

And I know—some of these theories may be new to you, but practicing new interpersonal communication skills and nonverbal communication skills is a sound form of communication training !

Check out this guide: 9 Conflict Resolution Tips to Win An Argument Like a Jedi

Popular Guides

12 thoughts on “10 effective ways you can improve your communication skills”.

communication skills research

Thanks Vanessa

communication skills research

Thank YOU for reading!

Danielle | Science of People Team

communication skills research

Micro-Expression: I have a few friends that I can read but other times it feels like a struggle. One “friend” in particular usually displays the disgust expression, hence I do not spend a lot of time with her. We are friends when I see her, but out of site out of mind…until now. What can I do to tune into the micro-expressions with others?

Great question. Some people have what is called a facial punctuator meaning they display a certain expression (surprise, contempt, disgust) as a way to emphasize a point or idea. Keep in mind that this does not necessarily mean that that this person feels this specific emotion more often, but instead, it’s part of their baseline. It may take a little getting used to!

Comments are closed.

How to Deal with Difficult People at Work

Do you have a difficult boss? Colleague? Client? Learn how to transform your difficult relationship. I’ll show you my science-based approach to building a strong, productive relationship with even the most difficult people.

Related Articles

Science of People offers over 1000+ articles on people skills and nonverbal behavior.

Get our latest insights and advice delivered to your inbox.

It’s a privilege to be in your inbox. We promise only to send the good stuff.

ORISE fellow hones science communication skills through BARDA fellowship

Meet shiraz belblidia.

ORISE fellow hones science communication skills through BARDA fellowship

Shiraz Belblidia is an ORISE fellow in the Office of the Director at the Biomedical Advanced Research and Development Authority.

Once a week while Shiraz Belblidia was in elementary school, a paleontology professor from a nearby college would enter her class and answer all the questions she and her classmates could think of. “He firmly believed that no question was too silly to be answered, and encouraged us as we tried to out-hypothesize each other,” said Belblidia.

The curiosity she fostered during her elementary school experience led her to pursue an undergraduate degree in biochemistry from Barnard College of Columbia University. Belblidia’s desire to understand the full scope and impact of biomedical research upon society motivated her to continue her education within an interdisciplinary master’s program at Georgetown University, where she was exposed to several global health topics including epidemiology, policy and governance. She graduated Georgetown University with a master’s degree in biomedical science policy and advocacy.

Belblidia is continuing her scientific pursuits within the Biomedical Advanced Research and Development Authority (BARDA) Research Participation Program, a fellowship hosted by an interagency agreement between the Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS), Office of the Assistant Secretary for Preparedness and Response and the U.S. Department of Energy (DOE).

As a current participant, Belblidia is a fellow in the Office of the Director, which engages in organization-wide operations and interfaces with each division and their endeavors. Because Belblidia’s fellowship is not assigned to a specific division, she has had the opportunity to collaborate with many groups, such as the Operations Team, Communications Team, Diversity, Equity and Inclusion Team and many more.

Under the guidance of her mentors Ashley Cecere, special assistant to the director, and David Howell, biologist, Belblidia has contributed to an outreach initiative for BARDA’s SPRINT (Science, Preparedness and Response, Innovations and New Technologies). This program serves as a resource for students from economically and racially diverse schools ranging from middle school to undergraduate level. Belblidia took the idea of the SPRINT program from concept to reality by engaging local schools and establishing opportunities for students to learn about potential career opportunities, fields of study and challenges individuals have overcome to get to where they are in their careers.

During her fellowship, Belblidia has honed her communication skills, project management skills and her ability to translate the mission of BARDA to real-world applications. “These are all incredibly important skills to me as a scientist and communicator, because I know that science does not and cannot exist in a vacuum. It’s therefore our (the scientific community’s) responsibility to make our work easily accessible to society,” she said.

Belblidia also stated that her time as a BARDA fellow has solidified her desire to use her extensive biomedical knowledge to approach global health issues. She has been inspired by the passionate scientists she is surrounded by. “This has been an incredible learning experience for me and to be able to not only witness, but participate in the processes that I had studied is honestly very empowering,” she said.

Upon completion of her fellowship, Belblidia will attend New York University’s Vilcek Institute of Graduate Biomedical Sciences and begin doctoral studies. She will be joining the microbiology, immunology and infectious disease training discipline. Her ultimate goal is to use the expertise she gains to participate and advocate for consequential biomedical research to promote global health concepts in the public, political and legislative arenas.

The BARDA Research Participation Program is funded by DHHS and is administered through the U.S. Department of Energy’s (DOE) Oak Ridge Institute for Science and Education (ORISE). ORISE is managed for DOE by ORAU.

  • Search Search Please fill out this field.

Problem-Solving Skills

Ability to work in a team, strong work ethic, analytics/quantitative skills, communication skills (written).

  • Skills on a Resume FAQs

The Bottom Line

  • Career Advice

Top Skills You Need on Your Resume

Yarilet Perez is an experienced multimedia journalist and fact-checker with a Master of Science in Journalism. She has worked in multiple cities covering breaking news, politics, education, and more. Her expertise is in personal finance and investing, and real estate.

communication skills research

The job market has its ups and downs but is always competitive. Because of this, it is imperative that you do all you can to set yourself above and apart from the competition by honing and highlighting your skills that are in high demand by employers. To help you on this path, below are the six skills and qualities most desired by employers.

The National Association of Colleges and Employers (NACE) included in their Job Outlook 2024 report the results of a survey in which it asked employers which of the skills and qualities they value most in candidates. The following are six of the most important, listed in the order of importance based on the results of the survey.

Key Takeaways

  • Employers receive thousands of resumes. To get noticed, yours has to stand out and include the skills employers desire in their employees.
  • Six of the most important skills, according to a survey by the National Association of Colleges and Employers (NACE) are problem-solving skills, the ability to work in a team, a strong work ethic, flexibility and adaptability, communication skills, both verbal and written.
  • When applying for a job, be sure your skills match the requirements needed for the job so as to not waste your time or the employer's time.

Problems will eventually arise in every organization, but how well you respond to these problems will determine how you set yourself apart from your peers and help the company that you work for. Making decisions that are in the best interest of the company, and solving problems with the most efficiency, while also limiting damages, are traits of a good candidate.

The ability to tackle a problem and overcome it and push your group and company forward is a trait not everyone has and one that companies seek the most in their candidates.

More often than not, your job will require you to work with others in order to get tasks and projects completed. This means that potential employers will want to be sure that you take kindly to sharing ideas, that you are open to ideas and input from others, and that you are willing to put the team and the company's interests ahead of your own.

Working hard is the name of the game. Working hard, but also efficiently, and putting in the extra work to do it well and not settling for the minimum is noticed by employers. A strong work ethic is the opposite of being lazy and one that is crucial to a successful career.

Every company measures its success based on numbers. The numbers that apply to you may depend on the department in which you work. For example, if you are in customer service , you may need to understand why more customers call during a certain period. Your ability to understand the statistics as they relate to the company can help you to implement plans that help improve efficiency and help the company to make more money.

In order to get work done, you may need to communicate with multiple departments in the organization. This can come in handy when you need to get something expedited for a customer, or if you need to understand how a function or process works.

The least important skill to have according to NACE's survey is fluency in a foreign language.

You will also need to effectively communicate with customers and vendors . An effective communicator is often a good motivator, which means you can get others to do their jobs. Being clear and concise in your work life is important to your success and to that of the company.

Companies love individuals that are able to take on a task themselves and push it forward without much hand-holding. Individuals that take an initiative and are able to bring people together to complete a task and make tough decisions are those who get things rolling in a company and continue to push it further.

Being someone who inspires others through their conduct and their work ethic are those employees that can take the company in a better direction.

What Skills Should You Put on Your Resume?

The skills you should put on your resume should be the skills that you have that pertain to the job. Such skills would include presentation skills, team-building skills, computer skills, leadership skills, problem-solving skills, and people skills.

How Many Skills Should You Put on Your Resume?

The number of skills you put on your resume should correspond with the skills you have and the specific job you are applying for. For example, if you are great at fixing cars but are applying for a computer programming job, then it wouldn't make sense to put the auto-mechanic skills on your resume. In general, having five to 10 skills on your resume should suffice.

What Are Professional Skills?

Professional skills are skills that directly demonstrate competency in a specific job. These are the skills that can be used in the workplace to get a job done, done well, and efficiently. Such skills include communication, presentation, and leadership skills. They also involve the specific technical skills needed for a specific job .

You should never forget that your resume is what will get you the interview , therefore it is up to you to sell yourself well enough so that when a potential employer sees it, they know right away that you are a good candidate for them. Make sure that your resume is up to date and showcases all of your skills and qualities.

When applying for a job, do your research so that you know the skills required by that employer. The importance of skills may vary among different employers and will also depend on the job for which you are applying. Be sure to highlight the skills for the job you want on your resume.

National Association of Colleges and Employers. " Job Outlook 2024 ." Page 26.

communication skills research

  • Terms of Service
  • Editorial Policy
  • Privacy Policy

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Environmental Factor

Your online source for niehs news, september 2024, undergrads share research projects from niehs summer internship.

Poster showcase builds presentation and communication skills, helping students who wish to pursue a career in the biomedical sciences.

By Erica Hinton

Eleven interns, who have been working alongside mentors at NIEHS, visually presented and explained their summer research projects as part of the July NIEHS Scholars Connect Program (NSCP) Summer Connection Poster Session.

“Posters are an integral aspect of science communication development for these young scientists,” said Undergraduate Research Program Manager Suchandra Bhattacharjee, Ph.D. “This format provides an opportunity for one-on-one presentations, in-depth discussions, and feedback.”

She added that the undergraduate students, representing five local universities, will have the opportunity to participate in two other events — in the fall and spring — to further enhance their science communication skills.

11 NSCP Scholars with Suchandra Bhattacharjee, Ph.D., front left, and OSED Director Ericka Reid, Ph.D., far right. Scholars from left to right: Giovonnii Nelson, Isabella Russ, Taymar Smith, Dhruv Ranganath, Faith Dee, Arnav Thakar, Olivia Hsu, Ricardo Scheufen-Tieghi, Emma Morgan, Coren O’Brien, and Justin Stegeman.

Congratulations to the following awardees, both from North Carolina State University.

Summer Connection: Best Presenter Emma Morgan Mentored by: Asmita Singh, Ph.D. and Carlos Guardia, Ph.D. Poster Topic: Exploring the Intracellular Incorporation, Distribution, and Accumulation of PFAS Using a Novel Fluorescent Probe.

Summer Connection: Honorable Mention Presenter Justin Stegeman Mentored by: Yesenia Rodriquez, Ph.D., Yang Liu, Ph.D., and Paul Wade, Ph.D. Poster Topic: Elucidation of Enhancer Activation: Poly-ADP Ribose Polymerase 1 Interactions With Transcription Factor GATA3.

Launched in 2012, the NSCP extends research opportunities at NIEHS to undergraduate science, technology, engineering, and math majors. Housed in the Office of Science Education and Diversity (OSED), a major tenet of NSCP is to increase opportunities for students from underrepresented groups to engage in research activities at NIEHS. Students with strong academic records are recruited from local colleges and universities for a three-semester paid internship (beginning in summer) designed to provide training in biomedical research.

The NSCP scholars will return in the fall and spring semesters, working part time at NIEHS while continuing to pursue their degrees.

Emma Morgan at poster session

(Erica Hinton is a contract writer for the NIEHS Office of Communications and Public Liaison.)

Related Articles

Participants exchanged experiences during the roundtable networking session. (Photo courtesy of Steve McCaw / NIEHS)

NC Women of Color Research Network elevates STEM leaders

Communication Challenge winners, from left to right: Kiara Smith, Saachi Gandhi, Aurea Michael, and Varun Kochar

NIEHS News in Brief

People in lab being taught proper pipetting

Biomedical research program for local teachers is a success

Yu-Ying Chen., Ph.D.

Scientific Journeys: Trainee’s science communication shines across NIH

Patricia Fletcher, Ph.D.

NIH offers training, resources to combat age discrimination

IMAGES

  1. Essay On Communication Skills

    communication skills research

  2. Improving your Communication Skills

    communication skills research

  3. Communication Skills

    communication skills research

  4. How You Can Use Academic Writing To Enhance Your Communication Skills

    communication skills research

  5. Communication is A Skill We Must Learn!

    communication skills research

  6. (PDF) Improving communication skills: Students' viewpoint on a content

    communication skills research

VIDEO

  1. Advanced Communication Skills

  2. 4. Research Skills

  3. 8613 Research Project Solved Autumn 2023 B.ed Aiou

  4. Writing Skills Interactive from Cambridge University Press

  5. Basic Communication Skills

  6. Motivation is essential for all round development of students, for details, @vipinkumartyagi5716

COMMENTS

  1. Performance-based assessment of students' communication skills

    Referred to as "performance-based" or "competence-oriented tests" within the field of competence research, such tests seek to represent holistically the individual's capabilities to act (Blömeke et al., 2015; Shavelson et al., 2018 ). Thus, even the designation of a "competence-oriented examination" of communication skills, for ...

  2. (PDF) Communication Skills in Practice

    modifying or even changing in behaviour. Specifically, communication is held to. share feelings and thoughts for several purposes that aim to connect with others. such as: inspiring, motivati ng ...

  3. Developing Effective Communication Skills

    The starting place for effective communication is effective listening. "Active listening is listening with all of one's senses," says physician communication expert Kenneth H. Cohn, MD, MBA, FACS. "It's listening with one's eyes as well as one's years. Only 8% of communication is related to content—the rest pertains to body language and ...

  4. Communication skills training for improving the communicative abilities

    Findings from qualitative research indicate that students learn communication and interviewing skills through the practice, observation, feedback and reflection that accompany simulation and role‐play activities, which Banach et al. found mapped onto Kolb's model of experiential learning. Further exploration of these issues is required.

  5. Communication Skills among University Students

    There are many types of communication skills, but generally it. involves oral and written skills. Mohd Helmi (2005) proposes that there are essentially three types of. communication, which are ...

  6. Perceived importance of communication skills and their predictive value

    Seven of eight communication skills identified in previous research (Burleson & Samter, 1990; Frymier & Houser, 2000) were perceived by students to be important in the teacher‐student relationship.

  7. PDF An Assessment of Students' Performance in Communication Skills A Case

    Communication Skills course at the University of Education, Winneba. The research also has an aim of bringing out suggestions and recommendations on how to improve the teaching and learning of communication skills. 1.3 Research questions The following questions guided the research: 1.

  8. Communication Skills among University Students

    Previous research on communication skills among university students have been reported by Ihmeideh, Ahmad and Dababneh (2010) and Cleland, Foster and Moffat (2005). They found that a positive communication environment provides opportunities to students to learn how to communicate, and thus, have better communication skills.

  9. 8 Ways You Can Improve Your Communication Skills

    These eight tips can help you maximize your communication skills for the success of your organization and your career. 1. Be clear and concise. ... Preparation also involves thinking about the entirety of the communication, from start to finish. Research the information you may need to support your message. Consider how you will respond to ...

  10. PDF Communication Skills among Undergraduate Students at Al-Quds ...

    interpersonal communication skills of graduates (Ronald, Quaid & Lindsay, 2005). 1.1 Literature Review In general, a lot of research on communication skills has focused on investigating the level of communication skills among university students (Iksan et al. 2010; Arywh, 2017; Ihmaidat, 2007; Ihmeideh, Ahmad, & Dababneh, 2010;

  11. PDF The Handbook of Communication Skills

    field of communication, offering a state-of-the-art overview of this rapidly evolving field of study. This comprehensively revised and updated fourth edition arrives at a time when the realm of interpersonal communication has attracted immense attention. Recent research showing the potency of communication skills for success in many

  12. The Importance of Communication in Research

    When the public is informed about your research, it increases the chances of them supporting the research financially and participating in studies or clinical trials. It helps you get the funding you need: Effective research communication can help you convince funding agencies that your research is worth investing in. A well-presented research ...

  13. PDF Skills for Effective Business Communication: Efficiency, Collaboration

    Communication in a social and cultural context with awareness of theme, roles, participants, situation and norms of interaction. Strategic Competence The ability to solve communication problems and compensate for deficiencies by verbal and non-verbal means.! Murphy Skills for Effective Business Communication HKS at Harvard University 30SEP14

  14. 8 Essential Leadership Communication Skills

    Essential Communication Skills for Leaders. 1. Ability to Adapt Your Communication Style. Different communication styles are the most frequently cited cause of poor communication, according to the Economist Intelligence Unit (pdf), and can lead to more significant issues, such as unclear priorities and increased stress.

  15. The Importance of Communication Skills [Top 10 Studies]

    2. Highly skilled communicators make more money. Research summary: According to a survey conducted in 1988 by D.B. Curtis and presented at the Annual Meeting of the Speech Communication Association says that the most valued skills in the "contemporary job market" are communication skills.

  16. Strong Communication Skills: Top 7 Traits to Focus On to ...

    Here's a list of the seven most crucial workplace communication skills with examples of communication competency in each and tips for developing them further. 1. Relationship building and maintenance. Human connection is fundamental to experiencing happiness, and genuine workplace connections improve one's mental health.

  17. Improving Communication in Clinical Research

    The second definition is broader and reflects the nature of communication more accurately. Poor communication in clinical research has many negative effects (Table 1) including stress, possible conflicts between clinical research professionals, and a breakdown in relationships. Other negative effects of poor communication are unmet expectations ...

  18. Why Is Effective Communication Important to Career Success?

    Why Communication Skills Are Vital to Career Success. Nearly every job posting contains the words "strong communication skills" or "effective communication skills." Good communication improves clarity in the exchange of concepts, knowledge, and ideas while reducing ambiguity or misunderstanding. This can affect businesses in a number of ...

  19. 10 Communication Skills for Your Life and Career Success

    Such details can be effective in communicating emotions and offer your audience insights into how others interpret your message. 7. Empathy. Having empathy means that you can not only understand but also share in the emotions of others. This communication skill is important in both team and one-on-one settings.

  20. (PDF) University Students' Communication Skills as a Determinant of

    Present research was to study the effect of the communication skills on th e students' academic achievement (CGPAs) at university le ve l. Fo llowing procedures were adapted for this study .

  21. Effective Communication Skills: Resolving Conflicts

    Even the happiest of relationships experience conflicts and problems (Markman, Stanley, Blumberg, Jenkins & Whiteley, 2004). If handled well, issues provide opportunities for personal and relationship growth. There are many skills that can help individuals seeking to resolve conflicts in a healthy way. One of the greatest skills that aids in conflict resolution is effective communication.

  22. 10 Effective Ways You Can Improve Your Communication Skills

    6. Use Powerful Words. Words are very powerful, depending on how we say them and the specific words that we say. In fact, using powerful words is one of the most valuable communication skills in the workplace because of the digital world we live in that simply requires us to look down and use our thumbs.

  23. The effect of teacher education supported through consultation on

    The purpose of this paper is to measure the effects of the consultation method that includes teaching and practicing the mand-modelling technique to teachers and parents to improve the communication skills of children with autism. Multiple probe design, which is one of the single-case research designs, was used in the study.

  24. ORISE fellow hones science communication skills through BARDA

    Belblidia is continuing her scientific pursuits within the Biomedical Advanced Research and Development Authority (BARDA) Research Participation Program, a fellowship hosted by an interagency agreement between Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS), Office of the Assistant Secretary for Preparedness and Response and the U.S. Department of Energy (DOE).

  25. Top Skills You Need on Your Resume

    Communication Skills (Written) ... When applying for a job, do your research so that you know the skills required by that employer. The importance of skills may vary among different employers and ...

  26. Half-million-dollar grant will create peer review and science

    More than 75 years of transformational research and hands-on social impact for a better world. ... that will immerse graduate students in the sciences in the "principles and practices of peer review and science communication with a heavy emphasis on building practical skills." Peer review is the system in which multiple experts review ...

  27. Communication Skills for Workplace Success

    communication skills within the workplace are, compatibility between jobs and skills, focus. on up-gradation of skills, perform well in interviews, organize group discussions on frequent. basis ...

  28. Leadership Behavior and Organizational Change Management in Selected

    In the Conceptual Framework below (see figure-1) the research objectives are integrated with the independent variable (leadership behaviors) and the dependent variables (organizational change management process), change initiatives implementation, and organizational performance.The academic community is identified as the specific group perceiving the leadership behaviors and their impact on ...

  29. Undergrads share research projects from NIEHS summer internship

    Eleven interns, who have been working alongside mentors at NIEHS, visually presented and explained their summer research projects as part of the July NIEHS Scholars Connect Program (NSCP) Summer Connection Poster Session. "Posters are an integral aspect of science communication development for these young scientists," said Undergraduate Research Program Manager Suchandra Bhattacharjee, Ph.D.

  30. Employability skills

    Communication skills also include non-verbal communication, such as the body language you use. Examples of ways that you can develop or improve your communication skills include: writing assignments and reports as part of your studies; using social media; making oral presentations as part of your class work